Skip to main content
Log in

Self-Perceived Gender Typicality, Gender-Typed Attributes, and Gender Stereotype Endorsement in Elementary-School-Aged Children

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined relations among self-perceived gender typicality, gender-typed attributes, and gender stereotype endorsement with a sample of elementary-school-aged children (N = 100, ages 6–12) from the Midwestern United States. Children who perceived themselves as more gender-typical were more interested in same-gender-typed activities and occupations and less interested in other-gender-typed activities and occupations than children who perceived themselves as less gender-typical. Gender typicality was linked to gender stereotype endorsement, as predicted based on Liben and Bigler’s (2002) dual-pathway model of gender development, with children who perceived themselves as less gender-typical having more egalitarian (less stereotyped) attitudes than children who perceived themselves as more gender-typical. The observed relations between gender-typed attributes and self-perceived gender typicality and between self-perceived gender typicality and gender stereotype endorsement did not differ across gender or age. These findings indicate that even young elementary-school-aged children use their knowledge of cultural gender roles to make subjective judgments regarding the self, and, conversely, that views of the self may influence personal endorsement of cultural gender stereotypes. Although the majority of extant research has focused on negative outcomes associated with low self-perceived gender typicality (e.g., low self-esteem), this research indicates that positive outcomes (e.g., flexible gender role attitudes) may also be associated with low self-perceived gender typicality.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amato, P. R., & Booth, A. (1995). Changes in gender role attitudes and perceived marital quality. American Sociological Review, 60, 58–66. doi:10.2307/2096345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, J. M., Bechtold, K. T., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2002). Who are tomboys and why should we study them? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 333–341. doi:10.1023/A:1016272209463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biernat, M. (1991). Gender stereotypes and the relationship between masculinity and femininity: A developmental analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 351–365. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bos, H., & Sandefort, T. G. M. (2010). Children’s gender identity in lesbian and heterosexual two-parent families. Sex Roles, 62, 114–126. doi:10.1007/s11199-009-9704-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, S. J., & Zucker, K. J. (1997). Gender identity disorder: A review of the past 10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 872–880. doi:10.1097/00004583-199707000-00008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, P. R., Yunger, J. L., & Perry, D. G. (2003). Gender identity and adjustment in middle childhood. Sex Roles, 49, 95–109. doi:10.1023/A:1024423012063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corby, B. C., Hodges, E. V. E., & Perry, D. G. (2007). Gender identity and adjustment in Black, Hispanic, and White preadolescents. Developmental Psychology, 43, 261–266. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drury, K.-M., Bukowski, W. M., Saldarriaga, L. M., & Santo, J. B. (2009, April). Gender typicality across social contexts. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, CO.

  • Egan, S. K., & Perry, D. G. (2001). Gender identity: A multidimensional analysis with implications for psychosocial adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 37, 451–463. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.4.451.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., & Taylor, M. G. (2000). Gender essentialism in cognitive development. In P. H. Miller & E. Kofsky Scholnick (Eds.), Toward a feminist developmental psychology (pp. 169–190). Florence: Taylor & Frances/Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halim, M. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2010). Gender identity and stereotyping in early and middle childhood. In J. C. Christler & D. R. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of gender research in psychology (pp. 495–525). New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-1465-1_24.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, G. (2000). Do gender role attitudes matter? Family formation and dissolution among traditional and egalitarian men and women. Journal of Family Issues, 21, 128–144. doi:10.1177/019251300021001006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreiger, T. C. (2005, April). Gender-atypical behavior in young children and its relation to social adjustment. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta, GA.

  • Lamb, L., Bigler, R. S., Liben, L. S., & Green, V. A. (2009). Teaching children to confront peers’ sexist remarks: Implications for theories of gender development and educational practice. Sex Roles, 61, 361–382. doi:10.1007/s11199-009-9634-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C., & Anderson, K. J. (1997). Gender development and heterosexual romantic relationships during adolescence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 78, 85–103. doi:10.1002/cd.23219977808.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C., & Bigler, R. S. (2011). Gender. In M. K. Underwood & L. H. Rosen (Eds.), Social development (pp. 289–315). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C., & Brown, C. S. (2008). Perceived experiences with sexism among adolescent girls. Child Development, 79, 685–704. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01151.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C., & Van, S. R. (2008). Masculine ideology, covert sexism, and perceived gender typicality in relation to young men’s academic motivation and choices in college. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 9, 139–153. doi:10.1037/1524-9220.9.3.139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, E. A. E., & Troop-Gordon, W. (2011). Peer processes and gender role development: Changes in gender atypicality related to negative peer treatment and children’s friendships. Sex Roles, 64, 90–102. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9883-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liben, L. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2002). The developmental course of gender differentiation: Conceptualizing, measuring, and evaluating constructs and pathways. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67 (2, Serial No. 269). doi: 10.1111/1540-5834.t01-1-00187

  • Lobel, T. E., Gruber, R., Govrin, N., & Mashraki-Pedhatzur, S. (2001). Children’s gender-related inferences and judgments: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 37, 839–846. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.6.839.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lowry, R. (2001). Significance of the difference between two correlation coefficients. Retrieved from: http://faculty.vassar.edu/lowry/rdiff.html.

  • Lurye, L. E., Zosuls, K. M., & Ruble, D. N. (2008). Gender identity and adjustment: Understanding the impact of individual and normative differences in sex typing. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 120, 31–46. doi:10.1002/cd.214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L. (2000). Cognitive theories of gender development. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 91–122). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Dinella, L. M. (2012). Congruence between gender stereotypes and activity preference in self-identified tomboys and non-tomboys. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 599–610. doi:10.1007/s10508-011-9786-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H. (1995). Children’s gender-based reasoning about toys. Child Development, 66, 1453–1471. doi:10.2307/1131657.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Fabes, R. A. (2001). The stability and consequences of young children’s same-sex peer interactions. Developmental Psychology, 37, 431–446. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.37.3.431.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L., Leonard, S., & Dinella, L. M. (2011). Experienced and expected similarity to same-gender peers: Moving toward a comprehensive model of gender segregation. Sex Roles, 65, 421–434. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-0029-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119–1134. doi:10.2307/1129498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 903–933. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Wood, C. H., & Little, J. K. (1990). The development of gender stereotype components. Child Development, 61, 1891–1904. doi:10.2307/1130845.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McHale, S. M., Kim, J., Dotterer, A. M., Crouter, A. C., & Booth, A. (2009). The development of gendered interests and personality qualities from middle childhood through adolescence: A biosocial analysis. Child Development, 80, 482–495. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01273.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Menon, M. (2011). Does felt gender compatibility mediate influences of self-perceived gender nonconformity on early adolescents’ psychosocial adjustment? Child Development, 82, 1152–1162. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01601.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. M., & Bigler, R. S. (2007). Effects of physical atypicality on children’s social identities and intergroup attitudes. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 433–444. doi:10.1177/0165025407081472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. M., & Pahlke, E. (2011). Student characteristics associated with girls’ success in a single-sex school. Sex Roles, 65, 737–750. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9904-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, D. G., & Bussey, K. (1979). The social learning theory of sex differences: Imitation is alive and well. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1699–1712. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickett, C. L., Bonner, B. L., & Coleman, J. M. (2002). Motivated self-stereotyping: Heightened assimilation and differentiation needs result in increased levels of positive and negative self-stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 543–562. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.82.4.543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Powlishta, K. K. (2002). Commentary: Measures and models of gender differentiation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67, 167–178. doi:10.1111/1540-5834.00201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, J. M., & Krueger, J. I. (2005). Social projection to ingroups and outgroups: A review and meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 32–47. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0901_3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (2002). Commentary: Conceptualizing, measuring, and evaluating the developmental course of gender differentiation: Compliments, queries, and quandaries. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67, 148–166. doi:10.1111/1540-5834.00200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). The development of gender-related constructs and content. In W. Damon & R.M. Lerner (Series Eds.) & N. Eisenberg (Volume Ed.) Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3, Social, Emotional, and Personality Development (6th ed., pp. 858-932). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

  • Scott, J. (2004). Family, gender, and educational attainment in Britain: A longitudinal study. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 35, 565–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Signorella, M. L., Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1993). Developmental differences in children’s gender schemata about others: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 13, 147–183. doi:10.1006/drev.1993.1007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Signorella, M. L., & Frieze, I. H. (2008). Interrelations of gender schemas in children and adolescents: Attitudes, preferences, and self-perceptions. Social Behavior and Personality, 36, 941–954. doi:10.2224/sbp.2008.36.7.941.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. E., & Leaper, C. (2006). Self-perceived gender typicality and the peer context during adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16, 91–103. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2006.00123.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T. (1984). Masculinity, femininity, and gender-related traits: A conceptual analysis and critique of current research. In B. A. Maher & W. B. Maher (Eds.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 13, pp. 1–97). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M. (1996). The development of children’s beliefs about social and biological aspects of gender differences. Child Development, 67, 1555–1571. doi:10.2307/1131718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tobin, D. D., Menon, M., Menon, M., Spatta, B. C., Hodges, E. V. E., & Perry, D. G. (2010). The intrapsychics of gender: A model of self-socialization. Psychological Review, 117, 601–622. doi:10.1037/a0018936.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Den Bergh, B. R. H., & Marcoen, A. (1999). Harter’s self-perception profile for children: Factor structure, reliability, and convergent validity in a Dutch-speaking Belgian sample of fourth, fifth, and sixth graders. Psychologica Belgica, 39, 29–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Volkom, M. (2003). The relationship between childhood tomboyism, siblings’ activities, and adult gender roles. Sex Roles, 49, 609–618. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000003131.98867.a1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, L., & Xie, D. (2010). Multidimensional gender identity and psychological adjustment in middle childhood: A study in China. Sex Roles, 62, 100–113. doi:10.1007/s11199-009-9709-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yunger, J. L., Carver, P. R., & Perry, D. G. (2004). Does gender identity influence children’s psychological well-being? Developmental Psychology, 40, 572–582. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.4.572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zosuls, K. M., Miller, C. F., Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Fabes, R. A. (2011). Gender development research in Sex Roles: Historical trends and future directions. Sex Roles, 64, 826–842. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9902-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zucker, K. J., & Green, R. (1992). Psychogenderual disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 107–151. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00860.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Sharla Cruse, Kelly English, Ban Hong Lim, and Rhea Owens for their assistance with data collection and Julie Milligan Hughes and Erin Pahlke for their feedback on earlier drafts of this manuscript. Preliminary results were presented at the biennial Gender Development Research Conference, San Francisco, CA, in April 2008. Final results were presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, CO, in April 2009.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Meagan M. Patterson.

Appendix: Gender typicality measure

Appendix: Gender typicality measure

Note. Items given are from the girls’ version of the measure. The boys’ version is identical except that the word girl is replaced with boy and the word boy is replaced with girl in all items. Response options are the same for all items. The gender typicality scale is part of a larger measure of gender identity that also includes measures of gender satisfaction, satisfaction with gender roles, and perceived pressure to conform to gender roles. The full measure can be obtained from the author.

Instructions: Some things are more typical of the groups they belong to than other things—they are better examples of the group. For example, a robin or a pigeon is a more typical bird than a penguin or an ostrich. People also have ideas about what girls and boys are like and who is a typical girl or boy. We want to know how you feel about yourself, and how much you are like most girls or most boys.

  1. 1.

    I like to do the same kinds of things as most girls.

    Really true

    Sort of true

    Sort of not true

    Really not true

    4

    3

    2

    1

  2. 2.

    I look like most girls.

  3. 3.

    I like to dress the same way as most girls.

  4. 4.

    Sometimes people say that I am acting more like a boy than a girl. (R)

  5. 5.

    I would rather do activities that girls usually like than activities that boys usually like.

  6. 6.

    I talk the same way as most girls.

  7. 7.

    Some things I like to do are things that boys usually like more than girls. (R)

  8. 8.

    I like the same school subjects as most girls.

  9. 9.

    I am good at the same things as most girls.

  10. 10.

    I would rather do activities that boys usually like than activities that girls usually like. (R)

Note. (R) reverse-scored item.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Patterson, M.M. Self-Perceived Gender Typicality, Gender-Typed Attributes, and Gender Stereotype Endorsement in Elementary-School-Aged Children. Sex Roles 67, 422–434 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0184-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0184-9

Keywords

Navigation