Skip to main content
Log in

On the functional form of temporal discounting: An optimized adaptive test

  • Published:
Journal of Risk and Uncertainty Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The tendency to discount the value of future rewards has become one of the best-studied constructs in the behavioral sciences. Although hyperbolic discounting remains the dominant quantitative characterization of this phenomenon, a variety of models have been proposed and consensus around the one that most accurately describes behavior has been elusive. To help bring some clarity to this issue, we propose an Adaptive Design Optimization (ADO) method for fitting and comparing models of temporal discounting. We then conduct an ADO experiment aimed at discriminating among six popular models of temporal discounting. Rather than supporting a single underlying model, our results show that each model is inadequate in some way to describe the full range of behavior exhibited across subjects. The precision of results provided by ADO further identify specific properties of models, such as accommodating both increasing and decreasing impatience, that are mandatory to describe temporal discounting broadly.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In addition, adaptive titration procedures have been developed, which adjust the amount or delay in response to the subject’s choices on a trial-by-trial basis, thereby reducing the risk of floor and ceiling effects (Mazur 1987; Johnson and Bickel 2002). Several studies have also employed adaptive heuristics in which stimuli depend on subjects’ responses. For instance, Abdellaoui et al. (2010) employed a two-stage design in which the first stage measured utilities, separately for each subject, which were then used in the second stage to construct stimuli for measuring time weights.

  2. Each mini-experiment could be a single trial or a block of several trials of trials.

  3. A geometric spacing of the independent variable (time) has been shown to be beneficial for discriminating between power and exponential decay curves in the study of memory retention (Cavagnaro et al. 2011). These sets of reward values were constructed to maximize the number of distinct ratios between the smaller and larger amounts.

  4. The following studies were consulted to develop reasonable expectations about the parameter ranges: Ebert and Prelec (2007); Frederick et al. (2002); Green and Myerson (2004); Killeen (2009); Laibson (1997); Loewenstein and Prelec (1992); McClure et al. (2004, 2007); McKerchar et al. (2009); Simpson and Vuchinich (2000).

  5. The raw AIC is an unbiased estimator of minus twice the expected log-likelihood of the model.

  6. Stephan et al. (2009) formulate this method in a Bayesian framework, using the posterior model probability in place of the Akaike weight.

References

  • Abdellaoui, M., Attema, A. E., & Bleichrodt, H. (2010). Intertemporal tradeoffs for gains and losses: An experimental measurement of discounted utility. The Economic Journal, 120(545), 845–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abdellaoui, M., Bleichrodt, H., & l’Haridon, O. (2013). Sign-dependence in intertemporal choice. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 47(3), 225–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82(4), 463–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1981). Preference reversal and delayed reinforcement. Animal Learning & Behavior, 9(4), 476–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akaike, H. (1976). Canonical correlation analysis of time series and the use of an information criterion. Mathematics in Science and Engineering, 126, 27–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Attema, A. E., Bleichrodt, H., Rohde, K. I., & Wakker, P. P. (2010). Time-tradeoff sequences for analyzing discounting and time inconsistency. Management Science, 56(11), 2015–2030.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benhabib, J., Bisin, A., & Schotter, A. (2010). Present-bias, quasi-hyperbolic discounting, and fixed costs. Games and Economic Behavior, 69(2), 205–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berns, G. S., Laibson, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2007). Intertemporal choice–toward an integrative framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(11), 482–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., Yi, R., Landes, R. D., Hill, P. F., & Baxter, C. (2011). Remember the future: Working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biological Psychiatry, 69(3), 260– 265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bleichrodt, H., Rohde, K. I., & Wakker, P. P. (2009). Non-hyperbolic time inconsistency. Games and Economic Behavior, 66(1), 27–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broomell, S. B., & Bhatia, S. (2014). Parameter recovery for decision modeling using choice data. Decision, 1(4), 252–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, K. P., & Anderson, D. R. (2004). Multimodel inference understanding AIC and BIC in model selection. Sociological Methods & Research, 33(2), 261–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2005). Neuroeconomics: How neuroscience can inform economics. Journal of Economic Literature, 43(1), 9–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, R. M., Meyer, J. R., & Huettel, S. A. (2010). Functional neuroimaging of intertemporal choice models. Journal of Neuroscience Psychology, and Economics, 3(1), 27–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavagnaro, D. R., Myung, J. I., Pitt, M. A., & Kujala, J. V. (2010). Adaptive design optimization: A mutual information-based approach to model discrimination in cognitive science. Neural Computation, 22(4), 887–905.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavagnaro, D. R., Pitt, M. A., & Myung, J. I. (2011). Model discrimination through adaptive experimentation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 204–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavagnaro, D. R., Gonzalez, R., Myung, J. I., & Pitt, M. A. (2013a). Optimal decision stimuli for risky choice experiments: An adaptive approach. Management Science, 59(2), 358–375.

  • Cavagnaro, D. R., Pitt, M. A., Gonzalez, R., & Myung, J. I. (2013b). Discriminating among probability weighting functions using adaptive design optimization. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 47(3), 255–289.

  • Chaloner, K., & Verdinelli, I. (1995). Bayesian experimental design: A review. Statistical Science, 273–304.

  • Dallery, J., & Raiff, B. R. (2007). Delay discounting predicts cigarette smoking in a laboratory model of abstinence reinforcement. Psychopharmacology, 190(4), 485–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, P. (2008). Discounting climate change. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 37(2-3), 141–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doyle, J. R. (2013). Survey of time preference, delay discounting models. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(2), 116–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebert, J. E., & Prelec, D. (2007). The fragility of time: Time-insensitivity and valuation of the near and far future. Management Science, 53(9), 1423–1438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O’Donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 351–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giordano, L. A., Bickel, W. K., Loewenstein, G., Jacobs, E. A., Marsch, L., & Badger, G. J. (2002). Mild opioid deprivation increases the degree that opioid-dependent outpatients discount delayed heroin and money. Psychopharmacology, 163(2), 174–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 769–792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., Fisher, E., Perlow, S., & Sherman, L. (1981). Preference reversal and self control: Choice as a function of reward amount and delay. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 1(1), 43–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2002). Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77(2), 129–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kable, J. W., & Glimcher, P. W. (2007). The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience, 10(12), 1625–1633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killeen, P. R. (2009). An additive-utility model of delay discounting. Psychological Review, 116(3), 602–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, W., Pitt, M. A., Lu, Z. -L., Steyvers, M., & Myung, J. I. (2014). A hierarchical adaptive approach to optimal experimental design. Neural Computation, 26(11), 2463–2492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, K. N. (1997). Bidding on the future: Evidence against normative discounting of delayed rewards. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(1), 54–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(1), 78–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koffarnus, M. N., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T., & Bickel, W. K. (2013). Changing delay discounting in the light of the competing neurobehavioral decision systems theory: A review. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99(1), 32–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kontsevich, L. L., & Tyler, C. W. (1999). Bayesian adaptive estimation of psychometric slope and threshold. Vision Research, 39(16), 2729–2737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kujala, J. V., & Lukka, T. J. (2006). Bayesian adaptive estimation: The next dimension. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 50(4), 369–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laibson, D. (1997). Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112(2), 443–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, X. (2008). The effects of appetitive stimuli on out-of-domain consumption impatience. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(5), 649–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindley, D. V. (1956). On a measure of the information provided by an experiment. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 27(4), 986–1005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65(3), 272–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (1992). Anomalies in intertemporal choice: Evidence and an interpretation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(2), 573–597.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G., Weber, R., Flory, J., Manuck, S., & Muldoon, M. (2001). Dimensions of time discounting. In Conference on Survey Research on Household Expectations and Preferences, volume 31. Paper Presented at Conference on Survey Research on Household Expectations and Preferences, Ann Arbor.

  • MacKillop, J., & Kahler, C. W. (2009). Delayed reward discounting predicts treatment response for heavy drinkers receiving smoking cessation treatment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 104(3), 197–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madden, G. J., & Bickel, W.K. (2010). Impulsivity: The behavioral and neurological science of discounting. American Psychological Association.

  • Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior, 5, 55–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S. M., & Bickel, W. K. (2014). A dual-systems perspective on addiction: Contributions from neuroimaging and cognitive training. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1327(1), 62– 78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306 (5695), 503–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S. M., Ericson, K. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2007). Time discounting for primary rewards. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(21), 5796–5804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKerchar, T. L., Green, L., Myerson, J., Pickford, T. S., Hill, J. C., & Stout, S. C. (2009). A comparison of four models of delay discounting in humans. Behavioural Processes, 81(2), 256–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106(1), 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, S. C., & Cusens, B. (2010). Delay discounting predicts increase in blood alcohol level in social drinkers. Psychiatry Research, 179(3), 324–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myerson, J., & Green, L. (1995). Discounting of delayed rewards: Models of individual choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64(3), 263–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myung, I. J. (2000). The importance of complexity in model selection. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 44(1), 190–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myung, J. I., Cavagnaro, D. R., & Pitt, M. A. (2013). A tutorial on adaptive design optimization. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 57(3), 53–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myung, J. I., Cavagnaro, D. R., & Pitt, M. A. (2016). Model evaluation and selection. In Batchelder, W. H., Colonius, H., Dzhafarov, E., & Myung, J. I. (Eds.) . New Handbook of Mathematical Psychology. London: Cambridge University Press.

  • Myung, J. I., & Pitt, M. A. (2009). Optimal experimental design for model discrimination. Psychological Review, 116(3), 499–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, J., & Büchel, C. (2011). The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: Understanding variability. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(5), 227–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, J., Miedl, S. F., & Büchel, C. (2012). Formal comparison of dual-parameter temporal discounting models in controls and pathological gamblers. PloS One, 7(11), e47225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. S., & Pollak, R. A. (1968). On second-best national saving and game-equilibrium growth. The Review of Economic Studies, 25, 185–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pine, A., Seymour, B., Roiser, J. P., Bossaerts, P., Friston, K. J., Curran, H. V., & Dolan, R. J. (2009). Encoding of marginal utility across time in the human brain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(30), 9575–9581.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pine, A., Shiner, T., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2010). Dopamine, time, and impulsivity in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(26), 8888–8896.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pitt, M. A., & Myung, I. J. (2002). When a good fit can be bad. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(10), 421–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. (2006). Notes on discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85(3), 425–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rangel, A., Camerer, C., & Montague, P. R. (2008). A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(7), 545–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ray, D., Golovin, D., & Krause, A. (2012). Bayesian rapid optimal adaptive design (broad): Method and application distinguishing models of risky choice. California Institute of Technology working paper.

  • Reynolds, B. (2006). A review of delay-discounting research with humans: Relations to drug use and gambling. Behavioural Pharmacology, 17(8), 651–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samuelson, P. A. (1937). A note on measurement of utility. The Review of Economic Studies, 4(2), 155–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scholten, M., & Read, D. (2006). Beyond discounting: The tradeoff model of intertemporal choice. LSE Research Online Documents on Economics 22710, London School of Economics and Political Science, LSE Library.

  • Scholten, M., & Read, D. (2010). The psychology of intertemporal tradeoffs. Psychological Review, 117(3), 925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharp, C., Barr, G., Ross, D., Bhimani, R., Ha, C., & Vuchinich, R. (2012). Social discounting and externalizing behavior problems in boys. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(3), 239–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shefrin, H. M., & Thaler, R. H. (1988). The behavioral life-cycle hypothesis. Economic Inquiry, 26(4), 609–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, C. A., & Vuchinich, R. E. (2000). Reliability of a measure of temporal discounting. The Psychological Record, 50(1), 3–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, K. E., Penny, W. D., Daunizeau, J., Moran, R. J., & Friston, K. J. (2009). Bayesian model selection for group studies. Neuroimage, 46(4), 1004–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Story, G. W., Vlaev, I., Seymour, B., Darzi, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2014). Does temporal discounting explain unhealthy behavior? A systematic review and reinforcement learning perspective. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(76), 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, T., Oono, H., & Radford, M. H. (2008). Psychophysics of time perception and intertemporal choice models. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications, 387(8), 2066–2074.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. (1981). Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Economics Letters, 8(3), 201–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toubia, O., Johnson, E., Evgeniou, T., & Delquié, P. (2013). Dynamic experiments for estimating preferences: An adaptive method of eliciting time and risk parameters. Management Science, 59(3), 613–640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bergh, B., Dewitte, S., & Warlop, L. (2008). Bikinis instigate generalized impatience in intertemporal choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (1), 85–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bos, W., & McClure, S. M. (2013). Towards a general model of temporal discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99(1), 58–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagenmakers, E. -J., & Farrell, S. (2004). AIC model selection using Akaike weights. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 192–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, X., & Dvorak, R. D. (2010). Sweet future fluctuating blood glucose levels affect future discounting. Psychological Science, 21(2), 183–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S. W., Filiba, M., & Camerer, C. F. (2010). Dynamically optimized sequential experimentation (dose) for estimating economic preference parameters. California Institute of Technology Working Paper.

  • Weiss, D. J., & Kingsbury, G. (1984). Application of computerized adaptive testing to educational problems. Journal of Educational Measurement, 21(4), 361–375.

  • Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (2004). Do pretty women inspire men to discount the future? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 271(Suppl 4), S177–S179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zauberman, G., Kim, B. K., Malkoc, S. A., & Bettman, J. R. (2009). Discounting time and time discounting: Subjective time perception and intertemporal preferences. Journal of Marketing Research, 46(4), 543–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel R. Cavagnaro.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

(PDF 231 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cavagnaro, D.R., Aranovich, G.J., McClure, S.M. et al. On the functional form of temporal discounting: An optimized adaptive test. J Risk Uncertain 52, 233–254 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-016-9242-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-016-9242-y

Keywords

JEL Classifications

Navigation