Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The relationship between language dominance and pre-reading skills in young bilingual children in Egypt

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study explored the relationship among pre-reading skills, language proficiency, and visual perceptual abilities in 150 children who had been exposed to both English and Arabic (mean age 70 months). Information regarding language proficiency was gathered indirectly through reports from teachers and parents and directly through children’s test performance. Consequently, the children were divided into three language dominance groups: stronger in Arabic, stronger in English, or equally strong in both English and Arabic. The main aim of this study was to determine whether the pre-literacy skills (visual processing skills, phonological processes, and orthographic knowledge) differed across the three groups and were correlated with langauge dominance. No significant relationships were found between language dominance and tasks that pertained to phonological awareness skills, which may support the notion of the transfer of phonological processing skills between languages. Visual processing and visual memory skills did not differ across language groups, a finding which was expected, as these functions do not directly pertain to language dominance. However, overall the groups did better on the English orthographic task than on the Arabic orthography, perhaps because they found Arabic print more visually confusing and demanding than English.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Graph 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abu Allam, R. M., & Hady, F. A. (1998). The peabody picture vocabulary test. Kuwait: Al-Bairaq Printing Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (2000). Effects of exposure to literary Arabic on reading comprehension in a diglossic situation. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13, 147–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (2001). The role of vowels in reading Semitic scripts: Data from Arabic and Hebrew. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 39–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S., Share, D., & Mansour, M. S. (2003). Word recognition and basic cognitive processes among reading-disabled and normal readers in Arabic. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 423–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (2002). Reading, syntactic, orthographic, and working memory skills of bilingual Arabic-English speaking Canadian children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 661–678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (2003). Reading skills in three orthographies: The case of trilingual Arabic-Hebrew-English speaking Arab children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 611–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Al-Mannai, H., & Everatt, J. (2005). Phonological processing skills as predictors of literacy amongst Arabic speaking Bahraini children. Dyslexia, 11, 269–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, C. (2006). Foundations of bilingual education and bilingualism. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beery, K. E., & Beery, N. A. (2006). The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration: Test of visual perception (5th ed.). MN: NCS Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S., & Leshem, H. (1993). On the interaction of phonologic awareness and reading acquisition: It’s a two-way street. Psychological Science, 2, 271–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy and cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2007). Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children: A framework for research. Language Learning, 57, 45–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., McBride-Chang, C., & Luk, G. (2005). Bilingualism, language proficiency, and learning to read in two writing systems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 580–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., & Swanson, L. B. (1991). Naming speed deficits in reading disability: Multiple measures of a singular process. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 195–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. M., Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., & Stevenson, J. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 39, 913–923.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Comeau, L., Cormier, P., Grandmaison, E., & Lacroix, D. (1999). A longitudinal study of phonological processing skills in children learning to read in a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 29–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (2000a). Language, power, and pedagogy: Bilingual children in the crossfire. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (2000b). Putting language proficiency in its place: Responding to critiques of the conversational/academic language distinction. In J. Cenoz & U. Jessner (Eds.), English in Europe: The acquisition of a third language (pp. 54–83). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test—revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu, A. Y., Nagy, W. E., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 453–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbeheri, G., & Everatt, J. (2007). Literacy ability and phonological processing skills amongst dyslexic and non-dyslexic speakers of Arabic. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20, 273–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eviatar, Z., Ibrahim, R., & Ganayim, D. (2004). Orthography and the hemispheres: Visual and linguistic aspects of letter processing. Neuropsychology, 18, 174–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feitelson, D., Goldstein, Z., Iraqi, J., & Share, D. L. (1993). Effects of listening to story reading on aspects of literary acquisition in a diglossic situation. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 71–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. (2005). Orthographic systems and skilled word recognition processes in reading. In M. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading (pp. 272–295). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographic depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 104–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genesse, F., Paradis, J., & Cargo, M. B. (2004). Dual language development and disorders: A handbook on bilingualism and second language learning. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Siegel, L. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Wade-Wooley, L. (1998). Component processes in becoming English-Hebrew biliterate. In A. Y. Durgunoglu & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Literacy development in a multilingual context: Cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 85–110). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., Wade-Wooley, L., & Shaney, M. (1993). The concurrent development of spelling and decoding in two different orthographies. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 383–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Schuster, B. (2000). Understanding differences in word recognition skills of ESL children. Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 123–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2000). Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia: Towards a cross-linguistic theoretical framework. Dyslexia, 6, 133–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goulandris, N. (2003). Introduction: Developmental dyslexia, language and orthographies. In N. Goulandris (Ed.), Dyslexia in different languages: Cross-linguistic comparisons (pp. 1–14). Philadelphia, PA: Whurr Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gutierrez-Clellen, V., & Kreiter, J. (2003). Understanding child bilingual acquisition using parent and teacher reports. Applied Psycholinguistic, 24, 267–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huguet, A., Vila, I., & Llurda, E. (2000). Minority language education in unbalanced bilingual situations: A case for the linguistic interdependence hypothesis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 313–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ibrahim, R., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2005). Is literary Arabic a second language for native Arab speakers? Evidence from semantic priming study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 34, 51–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ibrahim, R., Eviatar, Z., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2002). The characteristics of Arabic orthography slow its processing. Neuropsychology, 16, 322–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ibrahim, R., Eviatar, Z., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Metalinguistic awareness and reading performance: A cross language comparison. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 297–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lafrance, A., & Gottardo, A. (2005). A longitudinal study of phonological processing skills and reading in bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 559–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Saeigh-Haddad, E., Hende, N., & Ziv, M. (2008). Early literacy in Arabic: An intervention study among Israeli Palestinian kindergartners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 413–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacNamara, J. (1966). Bilingualism and primary education. A study of Irish experience. Edinburgh: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1995). What is phonological awareness? Journal of Educational Psychology, 8, 179–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Kail, R. V. (2002). Cross-cultural similarities in the predictors of reading acquisition. Child Development, 73, 1392–1407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicoladis, E., & Genesee, F. (1996). Word awareness in second language learners and bilingual children. Language Awareness, 5, 80–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulston, C. B. (1988). The international handbook of bilingualism and bilingual education. New York, NY: Greenwood Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reyes, I., & Azuara, P. (2008). Emergent biliteracy in young Mexican immigrant children. Reading Research Quarterly, 43, 374–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romaine, S. (1995). Bilingualism (2nd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Bilingual oral reading fluency and reading comprehension: The case of Arabic/Hebrew (L1)-English (L2) readers. Reading and Writing an Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 717–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2005). Correlates of reading fluency in Arabic: Diglossic and orthographic factors. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 559–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English-Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 481–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saleh, M., & Gamal, N. (1999). Spectrographic analysis of some Arabic vowels. Ain Shams Medical Journal, 50, 1473–1481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salim, A. K. (1991). Phonological and visual processes in word recognition by American learners of Arabic as a foreign language. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Conference on Reading, Palm Springs, CA.

  • Schatschneider, C., Carlson, C. D., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., & Fletcher, J. M. (2002). Relationship of rapid automatized naming and phonological awareness in early reading development: Implications for the double-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 245–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulz, E., Krahl, G., & Reuschel, W. (2000). Standard Arabic: An elementary intermediate course. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. (2003). Dyslexia in Hebrew. In N. Goulandris & M. Snowling (Eds.), Dyslexia in different languages: Cross-linguistic comparisons. Philadelphia, PA: Whurr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D., & Levin, I. (1999). Learning to read and write in Hebrew. In M. Harris & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 89–111). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, G., Bernard, C., Lalonde, R., & Rebai, M. (2006). Orthographic transparency and grapheme-phoneme conversion: An ERP study in Arabic and French readers. Brain Research, 1104, 141–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • State Information Service. (December, 2010). Education in Egypt. Retrieved from Egypt State Information Service website: http://www.sis.gov.e.g/En/LastPage.aspx?Category_ID=731

  • Taouk, M., & Coltheart, M. (2004). The cognitive processes involved in learning to read in Arabic. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 25–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • The Egyptian Cabinet Information and Decision Support Center. (2010). About Egypt. Retrieved from Egypt Information Portal website: http://www.eip.gov.e.g/AboutEgypt/GeneralInfo.aspx.

  • Umbel, V., Pearson, B., Fernandez, S., & Oller, D. K. (1992). Measuring bilingual children’s receptive vocabularies. Child Development, 63, 1012–1020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Versteegh, K. (2004). The Arabic Language. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, D. A. (1998). Putting second language first: Language and literacy learning in Morocco. In A. Y. Durgunoglu & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Literacy development in a multilingual context: Cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 169–183). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D., & Naglieri, J. A. (2006). Wechsler nonverbal scale of ability. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., O’Rourke, A. G., Gidney, C., Lovett, M., Cirino, P., & Morris, R. (2002). The second deficit: An investigation of the independence of phonological and naming-speed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 43–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the teachers, parents, and children of El Alsson British and American International School, Canadian International School of Egypt, The British Columbia Canadian International School, New Generation School, and Smart Village-Kipling School; and all who participated in this study; in addition to, Dr. Nadia Taibah and the Centre for Child Evaluation and Teaching in Kuwait for sharing their tests. The first author is particularly grateful for the support of the Sawiris Foundation Grant that made this study possible.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sandy Tahan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tahan, S., Cline, T. & Messaoud-Galusi, S. The relationship between language dominance and pre-reading skills in young bilingual children in Egypt. Read Writ 24, 1061–1087 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9301-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9301-3

Keywords

Navigation