Skip to main content
Log in

Differential effects of emotional expressions and scenes on visual search

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although research has shown that emotional content modulates attention, such studies often differ in the types of stimuli used to evoke emotions. Some studies utilize emotionally valenced scenes and others utilize emotional facial expressions. Importantly, the comparability of the effect of these two stimulus classes on attention is unclear. In the present experiments, we contrasted the effects of emotional scenes and facial expressions with the same valence on visual search speed. Overall, scenes caused greater disruption in visual search than faces, and emotional content appeared to modulate this effect with larger differences between scenes and faces arising for more negatively valenced stimuli. This pattern of findings was largely replicated after varying task difficulty in Study 2 and the visual properties of the search array and task difficulty in Study 3. These findings indicate that emotional scenes and faces produce differential effects on attention, and suggest that negative emotional scenes are particularly potent in disrupting the allocation of attention.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. These scenes were roughly matched for brightness and clarity. An independent sample of healthy controls (n = 20) rated each Anger (M = 51.71, SD = 27.96), Disgust (M = 42.50, SD = 23.34), Fear (M = 48.73, SD = 24.18), Happy (M = 39.53, SD = 20.69) and Neutral (M = 2.62, SD = 3.73) scene on arousal (0 = none to 100 = extremely/most imaginable). Emotional scenes (anger, fear, disgust, and happy) were found to be statistically higher on arousal when compared to neutral scenes (p’s < .05). Furthermore, the emotional scenes (anger, fear, disgust, and happy) did not statistically differ from each other on arousal (p’s > .05). Participants also completed a forced-choice task that required them to place each scene in one of five emotional categories: anger, fear, disgust, happy or neutral. Scenes where assigned to their predetermined emotional category the majority of the time: anger (65 %), fear (72 %), disgust (91 %), happy (93 %) or neutral (96 %). However, participants were more likely to mislabel anger scenes than other scenes.

  2. Scenes were presented for only 500 ms before the visual search task because time (100, 500 ms) was not found to significantly interact with stimulus (scene, face) or emotion (anger, disgust, fear, happy, neutral) in Study 1.

References

  • Alpers, G. W., Adolph, D., & Pauli, P. (2011). Emotional scenes and facial expressions elicit different psychophysiological responses. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 80, 173–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, T. J. (2005). Visual cortex: How are faces and objects represented? Current Biology, 15, R451–R453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armony, J. L., Servan-Schreiber, D., Cohen, J. D., & LeDoux, J. E. (1997). Computational modeling of emotion: Explorations through the anatomy and physiology of fear conditioning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 28–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arnell, K. M., Killman, K. V., & Fijavz, D. (2007). Blinded by emotion: Target misses follow attention capture by arousing distractors in RSVP. Emotion, 7, 465–477.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Awe, E., Serences, J., Laurey, P., Dhaliwal, H., van der Jagt, T., & Dassonville, P. (2004). Evidence against a central bottleneck during the attentional blink. Multiple channels for configural and feature processing. Cognitive Psychology, 48, 95–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar, M., & Neta, M. (2007). Visual elements of subjective preference modulate amygdale activation. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2191–2200.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, M. W. (2009). Panic search: Fear produces efficient visual search for nonthreatening objects. Psychological Science, 20, 435–437.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berggren, N., Richards, A., Taylor, J., & Derakshan, N. (2013). Affective attention under cognitive load: Reduced emotional biases but emergent anxiety-related costs to inhibitory control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 188.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276–298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., Moulder, B., & Lang, P. J. (2005). When good things go bad: The reflex physiology of defense. Psychological Science, 16, 468–473.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Britton, J. C., Taylor, S. F., Sudheimer, K. D., & Liberzon, I. (2006). Facial expressions and complex IAPS pictures: Common and differential networks. Neuroscene, 31, 906–919.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ciesielski, B. G., Armstrong, T., Zald, D. H., & Olatunji, B. O. (2010). Emotion modulation of visual attention: Categorical and temporal characteristics. PLoS One, 11(5), e13860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 203–216.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Controls of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 201–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2001). Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Perception and Psychophysics, 63, 1004–1013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., Kiss, M., & Cheung, T. (2010). Priming of pop-out modulates attentional target selection in visual search: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Vision Research, 50, 1353–1361.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finkbeiner, M., & Palermo, R. (2009). The role of spatial attention in nonconscious processing: A comparison of face and nonface stimuli. Psychological Science, 20, 42–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 355–379.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak, G., & Olvet, D. M. (2008). The persistence of attention to emotion: Brain potentials during and after picture presentation. Emotion, 8, 250–255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hariri, A. R., Tessitore, A., Mattay, V. S., Fera, F., & Weinberger, D. R. (2002). The amygdala response to emotional stimuli: A comparison of faces and scenes. Neuroscene, 17, 317–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodsoll, S., Viding, E., & Lavie, N. (2011). Attentional capture by irrelevant emotional singleton faces. Emotion, 11, 346–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in the human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–4311.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keil, A., & Ihssen, N. (2004). Identification facilitation for emotionally arousing verbs during the attentional blink. Emotion, 4, 23–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., Vanvolsem, P., & De Houwer, J. (2007). A time-course analysis of attentional cueing by threatening scenes. Experimental Psychology, 54, 161–171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kristjánsson, Á., Óladóttir, B., & Most, S. B. (2013). “Hot” facilitation of “cool” processing: Emotional distraction can enhance priming of visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 298–306.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krusemark, E. A., & Li, W. (2011). Do all threats work the same way? Divergent effects of fear and disgust on sensory perception and attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 3429–3434.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, N. A., & Bentin, S. (2008). Attentional and perceptual factors affecting the attentional blink for faces and objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 818–830.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P., Bradley, M., & Cuthbert, B. (2005). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual (Technical Report A-6). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundqvist, D., Bruce, N., & Öhman, A (2014). Finding an emotional face in a crowd: Emotional and perceptual stimulus factors influence visual search efficiency. Cognition and Emotion. doi:10.1080/02699931.2014.927352.

  • Lundqvist, D., Flykta, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces—KDEF [CD-ROM]. Stockholm: Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • McHugo, M., Olatunji, B. O., & Zald, D. H. (2013). The emotional attentional blink: What we know so far. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00151.

  • Milders, M., Sahraie, A., Logan, S., & Donnellon, N. (2006). Awareness of faces is modulated by their emotional meaning. Emotion, 6, 10–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Orienting of attention to threatening facial expressions presented under conditions of restricted awareness. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 713–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Miles, F., & Dixon, R. (2004). Time course of attentional bias for threat scenes: Testing the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 689–700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Most, S., Smith, S., Cooter, A., Levy, B., & Zald, D. (2007). The naked truth: Positive, arousing distractors impair rapid target perception. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 964–981.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, M. M., Andersen, S. K., & Attar, C. H. (2011). Attentional bias to briefly presented emotional distractors follows a slow time course in visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 15914–15918.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakayama, K., & Joseph, J. S. (1998). Attention, pattern recognition and popout in visual search. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.), The attentive brain (pp. 279–298). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Notebaert, L., Crombez, G., Van Damme, S., De Houwer, J., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Looking out for danger: An attentional bias towards spatially predictable threatening stimuli. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 1150–1154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fear, phobias and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olatunji, B. O., Ciesielski, B. G., Armstrong, T., & Zald, D. H. (2011). Emotional expressions and visual search efficiency: Specificity and effects of anxiety symptoms. Emotion, 11, 1073–1079.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a “low road” to “many roads” of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 773–783.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A., & LeDoux, J. E. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron, 48, 175–187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A., Ling, S., & Carrasco, M. (2006). Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Psychological Science, 17, 292–299.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Puce, A., Allison, T., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G. (1995). Face sensitive areas in the human extrastriate cortex studied by functional MRI. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, 1192–1199.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roelofs, K., Hagenaars, M. A., & Stins, J. (2010). Facing freeze: Social threat induces bodily freeze in humans. Psychological Science, 21, 1575–1581.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sabatinelli, D., Fortune, E. E., Li, Q., Siddiqui, A., Krafft, C., Oliver, W. T., et al. (2011). Emotional perception: Meta-analyses of face and natural scene processing. Neuroscene, 54, 2524–2533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Santos, I. M., Iglesias, J., Olivares, E. I., & Young, A. W. (2008). Differential effects of object-based attention on evoked potentials to fearful and disgusted faces. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1468–1479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schafer, A., Schienle, A., & Vaitl, D. (2005). Stimulus type and design influence hemodynamic responses towards visual disgust and fear elicitors. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57, 53–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U. (2005). Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: Threat, negativity, or arousal? Emotion, 5, 55–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt-Daffy, M. (2011). Modeling autonomic threat detection: Development of a face-in-the-crowd task. Emotion, 1, 153–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schubö, A., Gendolla, G. H. E., Meinecke, C., & Abele, A. E. (2006). Detecting emotional faces and features in a visual search paradigm: Are faces special? Emotion, 6, 246–256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. D., Most, S. B., Newsome, L. A., & Zald, D. H. (2006). An emotion-induced attentional blink elicited by aversively conditioned stimuli. Emotion, 6, 523–527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, T., Peelen, M. V., Funk, J., & Seidl, K. N. (2010). The fearful-face advantage is modulated by task demands: Evidence from the attentional blink. Emotion, 10, 136–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Susskind, J. M., Lee, D. H., Cusi, A., Feiman, R., Grabski, W., & Anderson, A. K. (2008). Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 843–850.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Hooff, J. C., Devue, C., Vieweg, P. E., & Theeuwes, J. (2013). Disgust- and not fear-evoking image hold our attention. Acta Psychologica, 43, 1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeulen, N., Godefroid, J., & Mermillod, M. (2009). Emotional modulation of attention: Fear increases but disgust reduces the attentional blink. PLoS One, 4, e7924.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: Neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 585–594.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weierich, M. R., Treat, T. A., & Hollingworth, A. (2008). Theories and measurement of visual attentional processing in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 985–1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, P. J. (1998). Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: Initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 177–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, E., Zald, D. H., & Blake, R. (2007). Fearful expressions gain preferential access to awareness during continuous flash suppression. Emotion, 7, 882–886.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zald, D. H. (2003). The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Research Reviews, 41, 88–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank David Zald for comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bunmi O. Olatunji.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Olatunji, B.O., Armstrong, T. & Ciesielski, B.G. Differential effects of emotional expressions and scenes on visual search. Motiv Emot 39, 589–601 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9477-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9477-y

Keywords

Navigation