Skip to main content
Log in

Early Pubertal Timing Mediates the Association between Low Socioeconomic Status and Poor Attention and Executive Functioning in a Diverse Community Sample of Adolescents

  • Empirical Research
  • Published:
Journal of Youth and Adolescence Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Low socioeconomic status (SES) may be associated with earlier pubertal timing and impaired attention and executive function (EF) in youth; however, whether pubertal timing mediates the relation between SES and attention or executive functioning remains unclear. Structural equation models tested concurrent and prospective relations between SES, pubertal timing, and attention and executive functioning measures in a gender and racially diverse sample of adolescents (N = 281, 45.6% male, 50.5% White/Caucasian, 46.3% Black/African American, 3.2% Biracial/other, and 44.5% low SES; complete data were not available on some measures). Youth from low SES families experienced earlier pubertal timing, and this accelerated development was associated with worse performance on attention and executive functioning tasks, both concurrently and longitudinally. These findings highlight a pathway by which youth from low socioeconomic backgrounds may develop worse attention and executive functioning abilities during adolescence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alloy, L. B., Black, S. K., Young, M. E., Goldstein, K. E., Shapero, B. G., Stange, J. P., & Abramson, L. Y. (2012). Cognitive vulnerabilities and depression versus other psychopathology symptoms and diagnoses in early adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41(5), 539–560. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2012.703123.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., Hamilton, J. L., Hamlat, E. J., & Abramson, L. Y. (2016). Pubertal development, emotion regulatory styles, and the emergence of sex differences in internalizing disorders and symptoms in adolescence. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(5), 867–881.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Arnsten, A. F. T., & Shansky, R. M. (2004). Adolescence: vulnerable period for stress-induced prefrontal cortical function? Introduction to Part IV. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021(1), 143–147. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1308.017.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Awh, E., Vogel, E. K., & Oh, S.-H. (2006). Interactions between attention and working memory. Neuroscience, 139(1), 201–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.023.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., Steinberg, L. D., Houts, R. M., Friedman, S. L., DeHart, G., & Cauffman, E., The NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2007). Family rearing antecedents of pubertal timing. Child Development, 78(4), 1302–1321. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01067.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: an evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62(4), 647–670. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131166.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Biehl, M. C., Natsuaki, M. N., & Ge, X. (2007). The influence of pubertal timing on alcohol use and heavy drinking trajectories. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(2), 153–167. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-006-9120-z.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S. J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 47(3-4), 296–312.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J. (1988). Antecedents and consequences of variations in girls’ maturational timing. Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 9(5), 365–373. https://doi.org/10.1016/0197-0070(88)90030-7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(3), 464–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(2), 233–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conley, C. S., & Rudolph, K. D. (2009). The emerging sex difference in adolescent depression: interacting contributions of puberty and peer stress. Development and Psychopathology, 21(2), 593–620. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000327.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Crews, F., He, J., & Hodge, C. (2007). Adolescent cortical development: a critical period of vulnerability for addiction. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 86(2), 189–199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2006.12.001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crossfield, A. G., Alloy, L. B., Gibb, B. E., & Abramson, L. Y. (2002). The development of depressogenic cognitive styles: the role of negative childhood life events and parental inferential feedback. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 16(4), 487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deardorff, J., Abrams, B., Ekwaru, J. P., & Rehkopf, D. H. (2014). Socioeconomic status and age at menarche: an examination of multiple indicators in an ethnically diverse cohort. Annals of Epidemiology, 24(10), 727–733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2014.07.002.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dorn, L. D., Dahl, R. E., Woodward, H. R., & Biro, F. (2006). Defining the boundaries of early adolescence: a user’s guide to assessing pubertal status and pubertal timing in research with adolescents. Applied Developmental Science, 10(1), 30–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, G. J., & Magnuson, K. (2012). Socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning: moving from correlation to causation: socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(3), 377–386. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1176.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, B. J., & Garber, J. (2000a). Psychosocial antecedents of variation in girls’ pubertal timing: maternal depression, stepfather presence, and marital and family stress. Child Development, 71(2), 485–501. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00159.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, B. J., & Garber, J. (2000b). Psychosocial antecedents of variation in girls’ pubertal timing: maternal depression, stepfather presence, and marital and family stress. Child Development, 71(2), 485–501. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00159.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, B. J., McFadyen-Ketchum, S., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1999). Quality of early family relationships and individual differences in the timing of pubertal maturation in girls: a longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 387–401.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Graber, J. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley, J. R., & Brooks-gunn, J. (1997). Is psychopathology associated with the timing of pubertal development? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(12), 1768–1776. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199712000-00026.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hackman, D. A., Gallop, R., Evans, G. W., & Farah, M. J. (2015). Socioeconomic status and executive function: developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental Science, 18(5), 686–702. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12246.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, J. L., Stange, J. P., Kleiman, E. M., Hamlat, E. J., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2014). Cognitive vulnerabilities amplify the effect of early pubertal timing on interpersonal stress generation during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43(5), 824–833. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-013-0015-5.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joos, C. M., Wodzinski, A. M., Wadsworth, M. E., & Dorn, L. D. (2018). Neither antecedent nor consequence: developmental integration of chronic stress, pubertal timing, and conditionally adapted stress response. Developmental Review, 48, 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.05.001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juraska, J. M., & Willing, J. (2017). Pubertal onset as a critical transition for neural development and cognition. Brain Research, 1654(Pt B), 87–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2016.04.012.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(2), 169–183. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logue, S. F., & Gould, T. J. (2014). The neural and genetic basis of executive function: attention, cognitive flexibility, and response inhibition. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 123, 45–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B. (2009). Developmental changes in cognitive control through adolescence. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 37, 233–278.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mac Giollabhui, N., Olino, T. M., Nielsen, J., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2019). Is worse attention a risk factor for or a consequence of depression, or are worse attention and depression better accounted for by stress? A prospective test of three hypotheses. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(1), 93–109.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magimairaj, B. M., & Montgomery, J. W. (2013). Examining the relative contribution of memory updating, attention focus switching, and sustained attention to children’s verbal working memory span. Child Development Research.

  • Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., & Robertson, I. H. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s attention: the Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(8), 1065–1081. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00806.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, D. P., Roediger, III, H. L., McDaniel, M. A., Balota, D. A., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2010). The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: evidence for a common executive attention construct. Neuropsychology, 24(2), 222.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McEwen, B. S., & Morrison, J. H. (2013). The brain on stress: vulnerability and plasticity of the prefrontal cortex over the life course. Neuron, 79(1), 16–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.028.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75(5), 1373–1386.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M., Nevado-Montenegro, A. J., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2012). Childhood executive function continues to predict outcomes in young adult females with and without childhood-diagnosed ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(5), 657–668. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9599-y.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Watkins, E. R. (2011). A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology: explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 589–609. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611419672.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ottowitz, W. E., Tondo, L., Dougherty, D. D., & Savage, C. R. (2002). The neural network basis for abnormalities of attention and executive function in major depressive disorder: implications for application of the medical disease model to psychiatric disorders. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 10(2). https://www-tandfonline-com.libproxy.temple.edu/doi/abs/10.1080/10673220216210. Accessed 22 Oct 2019.

  • Pechtel, P., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2011). Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: an integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology, 214(1), 55–70. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-2009-2.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117–133.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A., & Buhle, J. (2006). Typologies of attentional networks. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(5), 367–379. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1903.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repetti, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Seeman, T. E. (2002). Risky families: family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological bulletin, 128(2), 330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, I. H., Ward, T., Ridgeway, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994). The test of everyday attention (TEA). (pp. 197–221) Bury St. Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company.

  • Sarsour, K., Sheridan, M., Jutte, D., Nuru-Jeter, A., Hinshaw, S., & Boyce, W. T. (2011). Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: the roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(1), 120–132. 10.1017/S1355617710001335.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., & Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods of Psychological Research Online, 8(2), 23–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selemon, L. D. (2013). A role for synaptic plasticity in the adolescent development of executive function. Translational Psychiatry, 3(3), e238–e238. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2013.7.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, H. R., Miyake, A., & Hankin, B. L. (2015). Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00328.

  • Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28(1), 78–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stumper, A., Olino, T. M., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2019). Pubertal timing and substance use in adolescence: an investigation of two cognitive moderators. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(9), 1509–1520. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-019-00524-7.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sun, Y., Mensah, F. K., Azzopardi, P., Patton, G. C., & Wake, M. (2017). Childhood social disadvantage and pubertal timing: a national birth cohort from Australia. Pediatrics, 139(6), e20164099. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-4099.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth, N., & Spillers, G. J. (2010). Working memory capacity: attention control, secondary memory, or both? A direct test of the dual-component model. Journal of Memory and Language, 62(4), 392–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ursache, A., & Noble, K. G. (2016). Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context: multiple mechanisms and implications for measuring socioeconomic status. Psychophysiology, 53(1), 71–82. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12547.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Venkatesh, S. A. (1994). Getting ahead: social mobility among the urban poor. Sociological Perspectives, 37(2), 157–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Stumm, S., & Plomin, R. (2015). Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence. Intelligence, 48, 30–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.10.002.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children—fourth edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale—fourth edition (WAIS–IV). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson, 22, 498.

  • Wechsler, D. (2014). Wechsler intelligence scale for children—fifth edition (WISC-V). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wierson, M., Long, P. J., & Forehand, R. L. (1993). Toward a new understanding of early menarche: the role of environmental stress in pubertal timing. Adolescence, 28(112), 913–924.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants MH101168 and MH079369 to L.B.A.

Data Sharing and Declaration

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available but may be available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

A.S. generated hypotheses, created the database, aided in data analysis, and drafted the manuscript; N.M.G. helped generate hypotheses, ran and interpreted analyses, and helped draft the manuscript; L.Y.A. provided feedback on the manuscript; L.B.A. helped design the original study and write the grant that funded the study, participated in the design and coordination of this study, and provided feedback on all drafts of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lauren B. Alloy.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

The Temple University Institutional Review Board approved the protocol (IRB protocol #6844).

Informed Consent

Written informed consent was collected from all study participants after explaining their role in the study and before starting data collection.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stumper, A., Mac Giollabhui, N., Abramson, L.Y. et al. Early Pubertal Timing Mediates the Association between Low Socioeconomic Status and Poor Attention and Executive Functioning in a Diverse Community Sample of Adolescents. J Youth Adolescence 49, 1420–1432 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-020-01198-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-020-01198-x

Keywords

Navigation