Skip to main content
Log in

Simple Mindreading Abilities Predict Complex Theory of Mind: Developmental Delay in Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Theory of mind (ToM) is impaired in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). The aims of this study were to: (i) examine the developmental trajectories of ToM abilities in two different mentalizing tasks in children with ASD compared to TD children; and (ii) to assess if a ToM simple test known as eyes-test could predict performance on the more advanced ToM task, i.e. comic strip test. Based on a sample of 37 children with ASD and 55 TD children, our results revealed slower development at varying rates in all ToM measures in children with ASD, with delayed onset compared to TD children. These results could stimulate new treatments for social abilities, which would lessen the social deficit in ASD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A. L. (2015). The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42(2), 239–273.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-V (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Review Neuroscience, 7(4), 268–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W. (2001). The future of theory-of-mind research: Understanding motivational states, the role of language, and real-world consequences. Child Development, 72(3), 685–687

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1311–1320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30(2), 285–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Bowen, D. C., Holt, R. J., Allison, C., Auyeung, B., Lombardo, M. V., et al. (2015). The “reading the mind in the eyes” test: Complete absence of typical sex difference in ~400 men and women with autism. PLoS ONE 10(8), e0136521.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). The test for reception of grammar, version 2 (TROG-2). London: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broekhof, E., Ketelaar, L., Stockmann, L., van Zijp, A., Bos, M. G., & Rieffe, C. (2015). The understanding of intentions, desires and beliefs in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(7), 2035–2045.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, R. S., Hesketh, L. J., & Kistler, D. J. (2002). Predicting longitudinal change in language production and comprehension in individuals with down syndrome: Hierarchical linear modeling. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 45(5), 902–915.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1992). Understanding drawings and beliefs: A further test of the metarepresentation theory of autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(6), 1105–1112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 74, 195–203.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hill, J. (2007). Do children with autism have a theory of mind? A non-verbal test of autism vs. specific language impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 716–723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cornish, K., Rinehart, N., Gray, K., & Howlin, P. (2010). Comic strip task. Melbourne: Monash University Developmental Neuroscience and Genetic Disorders Laboratory and Monash University Centre for Developmental Psychiatry and Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denham, S. A., McKinley, M., Couchoud, E. A., & Holt, R. (1990). Emotional and behavioral predictors of preschool peer ratings. Child Development, 61(4), 1145–1152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised (PPVT-R). Forms L and M. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Pintilie, D. (1982). British picture vocabulary test. Windsor: NFER-NELASON.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenmajer, R., & Prior, M. (1991). Cognitive linguistic correlates of “theory of mind” ability in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 351–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L., Bates, E., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franco, F., Itakura, S., Pomorska, K., Abramowski, A., Nikaido, K., & Dimitriou, D. (2014). Can children with autism read emotions from the eyes? The eyes test revisited. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(5), 1015–1026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2014). Annual research review: Towards a developmental neuroscience of atypical social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(6), 553–557.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happe, F. G. (1994a). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happe, F. G. (1994b). Wechsler IQ profile and theory of mind in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1461–1471.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happe, F. G. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843–855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasegawa, C., Kikuchi, M., Yoshimura, Y., Hiraishi, H., Munesue, T., Takesaki, N., et al. (2015). Changes in autistic trait indicators in parents and their children with ASD: A preliminary longitudinal study. Psychiatry Research 228(3), 956–957. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2015.05.048.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1984) Early childhood autism and the question of egocentrism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 85–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(10), 389–398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kover, S. T., Haebig, E., Oakes, A., McDuffie, A., Hagerman, R. J., & Abbeduto, L. (2014). Sentence comprehension in boys with autism spectrum disorder. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 23(3), 385–394.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kumschick, I. R., Beck, L., Eid, M., Witte, G., Klann-Delius, G., Heuser, I., et al. (2014). READING and FEELING: The effects of a literature-based intervention designed to increase emotional competence in second and third graders. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1448.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2014). Autism. Lancet 383(9920):896–910.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laws, G., & Bishop, D. (2003). A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(6):1324–1339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a metarepresentational deficit? Cognition, 40, 203–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule, 2nd edition (ADOS-2) manual (Part I): Modules 1–4. Torrence, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazza, M., Lucci, G., Pacitti, F., Pino, M. C., Mariano, M., Casacchia, M., et al. (2010). Could schizophrenic subjects improve their social cognition abilities only with observation and imitation of social situations? Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 20(5), 675–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazza, M., Pino, M. C., Mariano, M., Tempesta, D., Ferrara, M., De Berardis, D., et al (2014). Affective and cognitive empathy in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(8), 791.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nordberg, A., Dahlgren Sandberg, A., & Miniscalco, C. (2015). Story retelling and language ability in school-aged children with cerebral palsy and speech impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 50(6), 801–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakes, A., Kover, S. T., & Abbeduto, L. (2013). Language comprehension profiles of young adolescents with fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 22(4), 615–626.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A. M., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill, M. M., Hutman, T., et al. (2010). A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(3), 256–266.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 415–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that …”: Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2005). Steps in theory-of-mind development for children with deafness or autism. Child Development, 76(2), 502–517.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Chimpanzee problem-solving: A test for comprehension. Science 202(4367), 532–535.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prior, M. R., Dahlstrom, B., & Squires, T. L. (1990). Autistic children’s knowledge of thinking and feeling states in other people. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 587–601.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sebastian, C. L., Fontaine, N. M., Bird, G., Blakemore, S. J., Brito, S. A., McCrory, E. J., et al. (2012). Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in adolescents and adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 53–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Harari, H., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Levkovitz, Y. (2010). The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in affective theory of mind deficits in criminal offenders with psychopathic tendencies. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 46(5), 668–677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Berger, B. D., Goldsher, D., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2005). Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cognitive and Behavioural Neurology, 18(1), 55–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sivaratnam, C. S., Cornish, K., Gray, K. M., Howlin, P., & Rinehart, N. J. (2012). Brief report: Assessment of the social-emotional profile in children with autism spectrum disorders using a novel comic strip task. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 42(11), 2505–2512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparrevohn, R. & Howie, P. M. (1995). Theory of mind in children with autistic disorder: Evidence of developmental progression and the role of verbal ability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 249–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland adaptive behavior scales (2nd ed.). Vineland: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Corbetta, D. (1994). Exploration and selection in the early acquisition of skills. International Review of Neurobiology, 37, 75–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, M. S. C., Annaz, D., Ansari, D., Serif, G., Jarrold, C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Using developmental trajectories to understand developmental disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 336–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Geert, P. (1991). A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth. Psychological Review, 98, 3–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vetter, N. C., Leipold, K., Kliegel, M., Phillips, L. H., & Altgassen, M. (2013). Ongoing development of social cognition in adolescence. Child Neuropsychology, 19(6), 615–629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., Solomonica-Levi, D., Shulman, C., & Pilowsky, T. (1996). Theory of mind abilities in individuals with autism, down syndrome, and mental retardation of unknown etiology: The role of age and intelligence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(8), 1003–1014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yule, W. (1978). Research methodology: What are the “correct controls”?. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 155–162). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author Contributions

MCP, MM, FF and MV designed research; MM and SP collected data; MCP, DD and FM analyzed the data; All authors have contributed to write the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maria Chiara Pino.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pino, M.C., Mazza, M., Mariano, M. et al. Simple Mindreading Abilities Predict Complex Theory of Mind: Developmental Delay in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 47, 2743–2756 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3194-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3194-1

Keywords

Navigation