Skip to main content
Log in

Low Fidelity Imitation of Atypical Biological Kinematics in Autism Spectrum Disorders Is Modulated by Self-Generated Selective Attention

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined whether adults with autism had difficulty imitating atypical biological kinematics. To reduce the impact that higher-order processes have on imitation we used a non-human agent model to control social attention, and removed end-state target goals in half of the trials to minimise goal-directed attention. Findings showed that only neurotypical adults imitated atypical biological kinematics. Adults with autism did, however, become significantly more accurate at imitating movement time. This confirmed they engaged in the task, and that sensorimotor adaptation was self-regulated. The attentional bias to movement time suggests the attenuation in imitating kinematics might be a compensatory strategy due to deficits in lower-level visuomotor processes associated with self-other mapping, or selective attention modulated the processes that represent biological kinematics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Prentice-Hall, NJ: Englewood Cliffs.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J., Simmons, A., & Williams, S. C. R. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11(6), 1891–1898. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00621.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bekkering, H., Wohlschlaeger, A., & Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 153–164. doi:10.1080/713755872.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Webb, S., & Murias, M. (2007). EEG mu rhythm and imitation impairments in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Brain and Cognition, 64(3), 228–237. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2007.03.004.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, G., Catmur, C., Silani, G., Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2006). Attention does not modulate neural responses to social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage, 31(4), 1614–1624. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2005). Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(2), 262–275.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, G., Leighton, J., Press, C., & Heyes, C. (2007). Intact automatic imitation of human and robot actions in autism spectrum disorders. Proceedings Biological Sciences/The Royal Society, 274(1628), 3027–3031. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1019.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(5), 843–859. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb01960.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1–2), 3–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buccino, G., Vogt, S., Ritzl, A., Fink, G. R., Zilles, K., Freund, H. J., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 42(2), 323–334. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00181-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W., & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(5), 667–684.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1982). The role of visual monitoring in observational learning of action patterns: Making the unobservable observable. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14(2), 153–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, J. L., & Bird, G. (2012). Atypical social modulation of imitation in autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1045–1051.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30. doi:10.1038/nn1611

  • Di Dio, C., Di Cesare, G., Higuchi, S., Roberts, N., Vogt, S., & Rizzolatti, G. (2013). The neural correlates of velocity processing during the observation of a biological effector in the parietal and premotor cortex. Neuroimage, 64, 425–436. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dinstein, I., Thomas, C., Humphreys, K., Minshew, N., Behrmann, M., & Heeger, D. J. (2010). Normal Movement Selectivity in Autism. Neuron, 66(3), 461–469. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.034.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, L. A. (2014). A meta-analysis of imitation abilities in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 7(3), 363–380. doi:10.1002/aur.1379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, D., Hansen, S., Grierson, L. E. M., Lyons, J., Bennett, S. J., & Hayes, S. J. (2010). Goal-directed aiming: Two components but multiple processes. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1023–1044. doi:10.1037/a0020958.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flash, T., & Hogan, N. (1985). The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model. The Journal of Neuroscience, 5(7), 1688–1703.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gangitano, M., Mottaghy, F. M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Phase-specific modulation of cortical motor output during movement observation. NeuroReport, 12(7), 1489–1492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gidley Larson, J. C., Bastian, A. J., Donchin, O., Shadmehr, R., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2008). Acquisition of internal models of motor tasks in children with autism. Brain, 131(11), 2894–2903. doi:10.1093/brain/awn226.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grèzes, J., Wicker, B., Berthoz, S., & de Gelder, B. (2009). A failure to grasp the affective meaning of actions in autism spectrum disorder subjects. Neuropsychologia, 47(8–9), 1816–1825. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, E. D., Battelli, L., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Repetitive TMS over posterior STS disrupts perception of biological motion. Vision Research, 45(22), 2847–2853. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.05.027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, E. D., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711–720.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2013). Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: A systematic review of current theories. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 91–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, A. F. D. C., Brindley, R. M., & Frith, U. (2007). Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1859–1868.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, A. F. D. C., & Grafton, S. T. (2007). The motor hierarchy: From kinematics to goals and intentions. In P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition: Attention and performance XXII (pp. 381–408). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. J., Andrew, M., Elliott, D., Roberts, J. W., & Bennett, S. J. (2012). Dissociable contributions of motor-execution and action-observation to intermanual transfer. Neuroscience Letters, 506(2), 346–350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. J., Ashford, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2008). Goal-directed imitation: The means to an end. Acta Psychologica, 127(2), 407–415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. J., Elliott, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2010). General motor representations are developed during action-observation. Experimental Brain Research, 204, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. J., Elliott, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2013). Visual online control processes are acquired during observational practice. Acta Psychologica, 143(3), 298–302. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.04.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. J., Hodges, N. J., Huys, R., & Williams, A. M. (2007). End-point focus manipulations to determine what information is used during observational learning. Acta Psychologica, 126(2), 120–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. J., Roberts, J. W., Elliott, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2014). Top-down attentional processes modulate the coding of atypical biological motion kinematics in the absence of motor signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1641–1653.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. J., Timmis, M. A., & Bennett, S. J. (2009). Eye movements are not a prerequisite for learning movement sequence timing through observation. Acta Psychologica, 131(3), 202–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2001). Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(6), 253–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic imitation. Psychological Bulletin, 137(3), 463–483. doi:10.1037/a0022288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C., & Bird, G. (2007). Mirroring, association, and the correspondence problem. In P. Haggard, Y. Rossetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition: Attention and performance XX (pp. 461–479). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and Identification in Autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(4), 649–659. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M. (2005). Neural mechanisms of imitation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(6), 632–637. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., Koski, L. M., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Woods, R. P., Dubeau, M. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2001). Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(24), 13995–13999. doi:10.1073/pnas.241474598.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286(5449), 2526–2528. doi:10.1126/science.286.5449.2526.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14(2), 201–211. doi:10.3758/BF03212378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (2007). Predictive coding: An account of the mirror neuron system. Cognitive Processing, 8(3), 159–166. doi:10.1007/s10339-007-0170-2.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception and Psychophysics, 21(6), 575–580. doi:10.3758/BF03198740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14(4), 334–339. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.14481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010). Automatic imitation of physically impossible movements. Social Cognition, 28(1), 59–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Longo, M. R., Kosobud, A., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2008). Automatic imitation of biomechanically possible and impossible actions: Effects of priming movements versus goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(2), 489–501. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.489.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E., Jr, Leventhal, B., DiLavore, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223. doi:10.1023/A:1005592401947.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nishitani, N., Avikainen, S., & Hari, R. (2004). Abnormal imitation-related cortical activation sequences in Asperger’s syndrome. Annals of Neurology, 55(4), 558–562. doi:10.1002/ana.20031.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perra, O., Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Fraser, L., Benzie, H., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Imitation and ‘theory of mind’ competencies in discrimination of autism from other neurodevelopmental disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(3), 456–468. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2007.09.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J. (1999). An examination of the imitation deficit in autism. In J. A. B. Nadel (Ed.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 255–283). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67(5), 2060–2073. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01843.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(5), 763–781. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3(02), 137–162. doi:10.1017/S0954579400000043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumiati, R. I., Weiss, P. H., Tessari, A., Assmus, A., Zilles, K., Herzog, H., & Fink, G. R. (2005). Common and differential neural mechanisms supporting imitation of meaningful and meaningless actions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(9), 1420–1431.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salowitz, N. G., Eccarius, P., Karst, J., Carson, A., Schohl, K., Stevens, S., & Scheidt, R. (2013). Brief report: Visuo-spatial guidance of movement during gesture imitation and mirror drawing in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 985–995. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1631-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 259–273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Southgate, V., & Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2008). Unbroken mirrors: Challenging a theory of Autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(6), 225–229. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spengler, S., Bird, G., & Brass, M. (2010). Hyperimitation of actions is related to reduced understanding of others’ minds in autism spectrum conditions. Biological Psychiatry, 68(12), 1148–1155. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, H. J., McIntosh, R. D., & Williams, J. H. G. (2013). A specific deficit of imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research,. doi:10.1002/aur.1312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., & Wheelwright, S. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(05), 747–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Théoret, H., Halligan, E., Kobayashi, M., Fregni, F., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Impaired motor facilitation during action observation in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Current Biology, 15(3), R84–R85. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vivanti, G., & Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2014). Imitation in autism spectrum disorders handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (4th Edn.). New Jersey: Wiley.

  • Vivanti, G., Nadig, A., Ozonoff, S., & Rogers, S. J. (2008). What do children with autism attend to during imitation tasks? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(3), 186–205. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2012). Social top-down response modulation (STORM): A model of the control of mimicry in social interaction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 1. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scales of intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wild, K. S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., & Gowen, E. (2010). The influence of goals on movement kinematics during imitation. Experimental Brain Research, 204(3), 353–360. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2034-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wild, K. S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., & Gowen, E. (2012). Goal-directed and goal-less imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1417-4.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. H. G., Casey, J. M., Braadbaart, L., Culmer, P. R., & Mon-Williams, M. (2014). Kinematic measures of imitation fidelity in primary school children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(2), 345–362. doi:10.1080/15248372.2013.771265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. H. G., Waiter, G. D., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D. I., Murray, A. D., & Whiten, A. (2006). Neural mechanisms of imitation and ‘mirror neuron’ functioning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 44(4), 610–621. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., & Singh, T. (2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 285–299. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000029551.56735.3a.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25(4), 287–295. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00014-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlschlager, A., Gattis, M., & Bekkering, H. (2003). Action generation and action perception in imitation: An instance of the ideomotor principle. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 501–515. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Robin Bush, Beverly Breen, and Hema Chandrashekhar for their help in supporting the study.

Author Contribution

SH conceived of the study, participated in its design and coordination, performed the measurement and statistical analysis, and drafted the manuscript; MA conceived of the study, participated in its design and coordination, performed the statistical analysis and measurement, and helped to draft the manuscript; DE conceived of the study, helped with the statistical analysis and drafting the manuscript; EG participated in the design of the study, and helped to draft the manuscript; SB conceived of the study, participated in its design and coordination, performed the statistical analysis, and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Spencer J. Hayes.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hayes, S.J., Andrew, M., Elliott, D. et al. Low Fidelity Imitation of Atypical Biological Kinematics in Autism Spectrum Disorders Is Modulated by Self-Generated Selective Attention. J Autism Dev Disord 46, 502–513 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2588-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2588-1

Keywords

Navigation