Skip to main content
Log in

Gaze Response to Dyadic Bids at 2 Years Related to Outcomes at 3 Years in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Subtyping Analysis

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Variability in attention towards direct gaze and child-directed speech may contribute to heterogeneity of clinical presentation in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). To evaluate this hypothesis, we clustered sixty-five 20-month-old toddlers with ASD based on their visual responses to dyadic cues for engagement, identifying three subgroups. Subsequently, we compared social, language, and adaptive functioning of these subgroups at 3 years of age. The cluster displaying limited attention to social scenes in general exhibited poor outcome at 3 years; the cluster displaying good attention to the scene and to the speaker’s mouth was verbal and high functioning at 3 years. Analysis of visual responses to dyadic cues may provide a clinically meaningful approach to identifying early predictors of outcome.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bahrick, L. E. (1987). Infant’s intermodal perception of two levels of temporal structure in natural events. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 387–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw, J., Shic, F., & Chawarska, K. (2011). Brief report: Face-specific recognition deficits in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(10), 1429–1435. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1150-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, D., & Dodd, B. (2004). Auditory-visual speech integration by prelinguistic infants: Perception of an emergent consonant in the McGurk effect. Developmental Psychobiology, 45(4), 204–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cantwell, D., & Rutter, M. (1994). Classification: Conceptual issues and substantive findings. In M. Rutter, E. Taylor, & L. Hersov (Eds.), Chid and adolescent psychiatry: Modern approaches (pp. 2–21). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chawarska, K., Klin, A., Paul, R., Macari, S., & Volkmar, F. (2009). A prospective study of toddlers with ASD: Short-term diagnostic and cognitive outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 50(10), 1235–1245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chawarska, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). Automatic attention cueing through eye movement in 2-year-old children with autism. Child Development, 74(4), 1108–1122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2012). Context modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(8), 903–913. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02538.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chawarska, K., & Shic, F. (2009). Looking but not seeing: Atypical visual scanning and recognition of faces in 2 and 4-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(12), 1663–1672.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chawarska, K., Volkmar, F., & Klin, A. (2010). Limited attentional bias for faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(2), 178–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum-Drucker.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, R. D., & Weisberg, S. (1982). Residuals and influence in regression. London: Chapman and Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8(4), 519–526.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duchowski, A. T. (2003). Eye tracking methodology: Theory and practice. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Efron, B. (1979). Bootstrap methods: Another look at the jackknife. The Annals of Statistics, 7(1), 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Efron, B., & Tibshirani, R. (1993). An introduction to the bootstrap. London: Chapman and Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Eikeseth, S., Klintwall, L., Jahr, E., & Karlsson, P. (2011). Outcome for children with autism receiving early and intensive behavioral intervention in mainstream preschool and kindergarten settings. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 829–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falck-Ytter, T., Gredeback, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9(7), 878–879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(14), 9602–9605.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gotham, K., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2009). Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(5), 693–705. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goutte, C., Toft, P., Rostrup, E., Nielsen, F. A., & Hansen, L. K. (1999). On clustering fMRI time series. NeuroImage, 9, 298–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, D. J., Colich, N., Iacoboni, M., Zaidel, E., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Dapretto, M. (2011). Atypical neural networks for social orienting in autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage, 56(1), 354–362.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (2002). Experience and brain development. In M. Johnson, Y. Munakata, et al. (Eds.), Brain development and cognition: A reader (pp. 186–216). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guellaï, B., Coulon, M., & Streri, A. (2011). The role of motion and speech in face recognition at birth. Visual Cognition, 19(9), 1212–1233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hastie, T., Tibshirani, R., & Friedman, J. (2009). The elements of statistical learning: Data mining, inference, and prediction. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P. (2005). Outcomes in autism spectrum disorders. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders: Diagnosis, development, neurobiology, and behavior (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 201–220). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubert, L., & Arabie, P. (1985). Comparing partitions. Journal of Classification, 2(1), 193–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H., Grossmann, T., & Kadosh, K. C. (2009). Mapping functional brain development: Building a social brain through interactive specialization. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 151–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(8), 946–954.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M. J., Brown, C. A., Lee, C. T., Cowperthwaite, B., Stutzman, S. S., et al. (2009). Fetal sensitivity to properties of maternal speech and language. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(1), 59–71. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. In U. Frith & E. Hill (Eds.), Autism: Mind and brain (pp. 127–159). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Saulnier, C. A., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., Volkmar, F. R., & Lord, C. (2007). Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: The Vineland and the ADOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 748–759.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klintwall, L., & Eikeseth, S. (2012). Number and controllability of reinforcers as predictors of individual outcome for children with autism receiving early and intensive behavioral intervention. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 493–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landa, R. J., Holman, K. C., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (2007). Social and communication development in toddlers with early and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(7), 853–864.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lansing, C. R., & McConkie, G. W. (1999). Attention to facial regions in segmental and prosodic visual speech perception tasks. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 42(3), 526–539.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewkowicz, D. J. (2000). The development of intersensory temporal perception: An epigenetic systems/limitations view. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 2281–2308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewkowicz, D. J., & Hansen-Tift, A. M. (2012). Infants deploy selective attention to the mouth of a talking face when learning speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(5), 1431–1436. doi:10.1073/pnas.1114783109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., DiLavore, P. S., Shulman, C., Thurm, A., & Pickles, A. (2006). Autism from 2 to 9 years of age. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(6), 694–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2000). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation scale-2. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • MathWorks. (2009). MATLAB 7.8 (Release R2009a). Retrieved from http://www.mathworks.com.

  • Morris, S. B., & DeShon, R. P. (2002). Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychological Methods, 7, 105–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning, AGS edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Serivce, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadig, A. S., Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Rozga, A., Sigman, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2007). A prospective study of response to name in infants at risk for autism. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 161(4), 378–383.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C. F., Brock, J., Cragg, L., Einav, S., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2009). Eye-movement patterns are associated with communicative competence in autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(7), 834–842.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, R., Chawarska, K., Fowler, C., Cicchetti, D., & Volkmar, F. (2007). Listen my children and you shall hear: Auditory preferences in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(5), 1350–1364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, A., Cummings, A., Geier, J. D., Freeman, N. L., Hughes, S., Managhan, T., et al. (2011). Predictors of outcome for children receiving intensive behavioral intervention in a large, community-based program. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 592–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, K., Conant, D., Hazin, R., Stoner, R., & Desmond, J. (2011). Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(1), 101–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • R Development Core Team. (2011). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org.

  • Sai, F. Z. (2005). The role of the mother’s voice in developing mother’s face preference: Evidence for intermodal perception at birth. Infant & Child Development, 14(1), 29–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SAS Institute Inc. (2002–2010). SAS proprietary software 9.3, Cary, NC.

  • Sherer, M. R., & Schreibman, L. (2005). Individual behavioral profiles and predictors of treatment effectiveness for children with autism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(3), 525–538.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shic, F. (2008). Computational methods for eye-tracking analysis: Applications to autism. Computer science. New Haven, CT: Yale University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shic, F., Bradshaw, J., Klin, A., Scassellati, B., & Chawarska, K. (2010). Limited activity monitoring in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Research, 1380, 246–254.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (2005). Vineland adaptive behavior scales, (Vineland-II) (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teinonen, T., Aslin, R. N., Alku, P., & Csibra, G. (2008). Visual speech contributes to phonetic learning in 6-month-old infants. Cognition, 108(3), 850–855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tibshirani, R., Walther, G., & Hastie, T. (2001). Estimating the number of clusters in a dataset via the gap statistic. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B, 32(2), 411–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsao, F.-M., Liu, H.-M., & Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 75(4), 1067–1084.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vouloumanos, A., Hauser, M. D., Werker, J. F., & Martin, A. (2010). The tuning of human neonates’ preference for speech. Child Development, 81(2), 517–527. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01412.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, J. H., Jr. (1963). Hierarchical grouping to optimize an objective function. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 58(301), 236–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, G. S., Merin, N., Rogers, S. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: Predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Developmental Science, 12(5), 798–814. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00833.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The study was supported by the National Alliance for Autism Research Foundation, Autism Speaks Foundation, NICHD ACE Grant P50 MH081756-0, NIMH Grants 1R03MH086732, and R03 MH092618-01A1. We would also like to thank Celine Saulnier, Amanda Steiner, Karyn Bailey, and Rhea Paul for their contribution to the sample characterization as well as Jessica Bradshaw, Mairin Meltvedt, Grace Chen, Marika Coffman, Alexandra Dowd, Eugenia Gisin, and Jessica Reed for assistance in data collection. We wish to express our appreciation to the families and their children for their time and participation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Katarzyna Chawarska.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Campbell, D.J., Shic, F., Macari, S. et al. Gaze Response to Dyadic Bids at 2 Years Related to Outcomes at 3 Years in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Subtyping Analysis. J Autism Dev Disord 44, 431–442 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1885-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1885-9

Keywords

Navigation