Skip to main content
Log in

Differential Genetic and Environmental Influences on Reactive and Proactive Aggression in Children

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

While significant heritability for childhood aggression has been claimed, it is not known whether there are differential genetic and environmental contributions to proactive and reactive forms of aggression in children. This study quantifies genetic and environmental contributions to these two forms of aggression in an ethnically diverse urban sample of 9–10 year old twins (N = 1219), and compares results across different informants (child self-report, mother, and teacher ratings) using the Reactive–Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ). Confirmatory factor analysis of RPQ items indicated a significant and strong fit for a two-factor proactive–reactive model which was significantly superior to a one-factor model and which replicated across gender as well as the three informant sources. Males scored significantly higher than females on both self-report reactive and proactive aggression, findings that replicated on mother and teacher versions of the RPQ. Asian–Americans scored lower than most ethnic groups on reactive aggression yet were equivalent to Caucasians on proactive aggression. African–Americans scored higher than other ethnic groups on all measures of aggression except caregiver reports. Heritable influences were found for both forms of aggression across informants, but while boys’ self-reports revealed genetic influences on proactive (50%) and reactive (38%) aggression, shared and non-shared environmental influences almost entirely accounted for girls’ self-report reactive and proactive aggression. Although genetic correlations between reactive and proactive aggression were significant across informants, there was evidence that the genetic correlation was less than unity in boys self reported aggression, indicating that genetic factors differ for proactive and reactive aggression. These findings provide the first evidence for varying genetic and environmental etiologies for reactive and proactive aggression across gender, and provide additional support for distinction between these two forms of aggression.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: Implications of cross informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Akaike, A. C. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. A., Barton, M., & Raine, A. (2002). The Southern California twin register at the University of Southern California. Twin Research, 5, 456–459.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L.A., Barton, M., Lozano, D.I., Raine, A., Fowler, J. H. (2006). The Southern California twin register at the University of Southern California: II. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9(6), 933–940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. A., Jacobsen, K., Raine, A., Lozano, D. I., & Bezdjian, S. (2007). Genetic and environmental bases of antisocial behavior in children: I A multi-informant twin study Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(2), 219–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1983). Psychological mechanisms of aggression. In R. G. Green & E. I. Donnerstein (Eds.), Aggression: Theoretical and empirical reviews (vol. 1, pp. 1–40). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS Structural equations program manual. Encino: Multivariate Software.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M., Berkane, M., & Kano, Y. (1991). Covariance structure analysis under a simple kurtosis model. In E. M. Keramidas (Ed.), Computing science and statistics (pp. 463–465). Fairfax Station: Interface Foundation of North America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88(3), 588–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M., & Dudgeon, P. (1996). Covariance structure analysis: statistical practice, theory, and directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 563–592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M., & Wu, E. J. C. (2002). EQS 6 for windows users guide. Encino: Multivariate Software.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L. (1963). Aggression: A social psychological analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., & Perusse, D. (2006). Examining genetic and environmental effects on reactive versus proactive aggression. Development Psychology, 42, 1299–1312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2001). Is it time to pull the plug on the hostile versus instrumental aggression dichotomy. Psychological Review, 108, 273–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cadoret, R. J., Leve, L. D., & Devor, E. (1997). Genetics of aggressive and violent behavior. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 20, 301–322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms on reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67, 993–1002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. A. (1991). The structure and function of reactive and proactive aggression. In D. Pepler & K. Rubin (Eds.), The development and treatment for childhood aggression (pp. 201–218). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 53, 1146–1158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. A., Lochman, J. E., Harnish, J. D., & Bates, J. E. (1997a). Reactive and proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 37–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1997b). How the experience of early physical abuse leads children to become chronically aggressive. In D. En Cicchetti, & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Developmental perspectives on trauma: theory, research, and intervention (pp. 263–288). Rochester: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelbrock, C., Rende, R., Plomin, R., & Thompson, L. A. (1995). A twin study of competence and problem behavior in childhood and early adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 36, 775–785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grafman, J., Schwab, K., Warden, D., Pridgen, A., Brown, H. R., & Salazar, A. M. (1996). Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression: a report of the Vietnam head injury study. Neurology, 46, 1231–1238.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gregg, T.R., & Siegel, A. (2001). Brain structures and neurotransmitters regulating aggression in cats: Implications for human aggression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 25(1), 91–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudziak, J. J., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Bartels, M., Rietveld, M. J. H., Rettew, D. C., & Derks, E. M. (2003). Individual differences in aggression: genetic analyses by age, gender, and informant in 3-, 7-, and 10-year-old Dutch twins. Behavior Genetics, 33, 575–589.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. (1992). Yesterday’s premises, tomorrow’s promises. Developmental Psychology, 28, 990–997.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy: temperament in human nature. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kano, Y., Berkane, M., & Bentler, P. M. (1990). Covariance structure analysis with heterogeneous kurtosis parameters. Biometrika, 77, 575–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempes, M., Matthys, W., Maassen, G., van Goozen, S., & van Engeland, H. (2006). A parent questionnaire for distinguishing between reactive and proactive aggression in children. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 15, 38–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laird, R. D., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. E. (2005). Peer relationship antecedents of delinquent behavior in late adolescence: is there evidence of demographic group differences in developmental processes? Development and Psychopathology, 17(1), 127–144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Latent variable models: An introduction to factor, path, and structural analysis (p. 292). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, R. P. (1989). An index of goodness-of-fit based on noncentrality. Journal of Classification, 6, 97–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, D. R., & Carey, G. (1997). Genetic and environmental architecture on human aggression. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 72, 207–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, K. A., Hilt, L. M., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2007). Racial/ethnic differences in internalizing and externalizing symptoms in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(5), 801–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meloy, J.R. (1988). The psychopathic mind: Origins, dynamics, and treatment. Northvale: Jason Aronson.

  • Patterson, G. R. (2002). The early development of coercive family processes. In J. B. Reid, G. R. Patterson, & J. Snyder (Eds.), Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental analysis and model for intervention (pp. 25–44). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R., Nitz, K., & Rowe, D. C. (1990). Behavioral genetics and aggressive behavior in childhood. In M. Lewis & S. M. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology: Perspectives in developmental psychology (pp. 119–133). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poulin, F., & Boivin, M. (2000). Reactive and proactive aggression: Evidence of a two-factor model. Psychological Assessment, 12, 115–122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poulin, F., Cillessen, A. H. N., Hubbard, J. A., Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Schwartz, D. (1997). Children’s friends and behavioral similarity in two social contexts. Social development, 6, 224–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price, J. M., & Dodge, K. A. (1989). Reactive and proactive aggression in childhood: relations to peer status and social context dimensions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 455–471.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raine, A., Dodge, K., Loeber, R., Gatzke-Kopp, L., Lynam, D., & Reynolds, C. (2006). The Reactive-Proactive Aggression (RPQ) questionnaire: differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 159–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raine, A., Meloy, J. R., Bihrle, S., Stoddard, J., LaCasse, L., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1998). Reduced prefrontal and increased subcortical brain functioning assessed using positron emission tomography in predatory and affective murderers. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 16, 319–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Satorra, P. M., & Bentler, A. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66, 507–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Meyer, J., Silberg, J., & Maes, H. (1998). Genetic and environmental influences on subtypes of conduct disorder behavior in boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 495–509.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P. M., Cannon, T. D., Narr, K. L., van Erp, T., Poutanen, V. P., & Huttunen, M. (2001). Genetic influences on brain structure. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 1253–1258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomada, G., & Schneider, B. H. (1997). Relational aggression, gender, and peer acceptance: Invariance across culture, stability over time, and concordance among informants. Developmental Psychology, 33, 601–609.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by grants to the first author from National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH; RO1 MH58354), to the second author from NIMH (Independent Scientist Award K02 MH01114-08), to the third author from an National Institute of Nursing Research post-doctoral fellowship award (F32 NR-08661-2) and National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (K01 ES015877-01), and to the fourth author from National Institutes of Health (K01 MH068484-01).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura A. Baker.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Baker, L.A., Raine, A., Liu, J. et al. Differential Genetic and Environmental Influences on Reactive and Proactive Aggression in Children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 36, 1265–1278 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9249-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9249-1

Keywords

Navigation