Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Working Memory Underpins Cognitive Development, Learning, and Education

  • Review Article
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Working memory is the retention of a small amount of information in a readily accessible form. It facilitates planning, comprehension, reasoning, and problem solving. I examine the historical roots and conceptual development of the concept and the theoretical and practical implications of current debates about working memory mechanisms. Then, I explore the nature of cognitive developmental improvements in working memory, the role of working memory in learning, and some potential implications of working memory and its development for the education of children and adults. The use of working memory is quite ubiquitous in human thought, but the best way to improve education using what we know about working memory is still controversial. I hope to provide some directions for research and educational practice.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andrews, G., & Halford, G. S. (2002). A cognitive complexity metric applied to cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 153–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: a proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford Psychology Series #11. Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrouillet, P., Gavens, N., Vergauwe, E., Gaillard, V., & Camos, V. (2009). Working memory span development: a time-based resource-sharing model account. Developmental Psychology, 45, 477–490.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrouillet, P., Portrat, S., & Camos, V. (2011). On the law relating processing to storage in working memory. Psychological Review, 118, 175–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, P. J., & Fivush, R. (2013). Handbook on the development of children’s memory. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolton, T. L. (1892). The growth of memory in school children. American Journal of Psychology, 4, 362–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borst, G., Niven, E., & Logie, R. H. (2012). Visual mental image generation does not overlap with visual short-term memory: a dual-task interference study. Memory & cognition, 40, 360–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. D. A., Neath, I., & Chater, N. (2007). A temporal ratio model of memory. Psychological Review, 114, 539–576.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: a network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106, 551–581.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camos, V., & Barrouillet, P. (2011). Developmental change in working memory strategies: from passive maintenance to active refreshing. Developmental Psychology, 47, 898–904.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camos, V., Mora, G., & Oberauer, K. (2011). Adaptive choice between articulatory rehearsal and attentional refreshing in verbal working memory. Memory & Cognition, 39, 231–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., Kurland, D. M., & Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 386–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chein, J., & Morrison, A. (2010). Expanding the mind’s workspace: training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 193–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Z., & Cowan, N. (2009). Core verbal working memory capacity: the limit in words retained without covert articulation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1420–1429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Z., & Cowan, N. (2013). Working memory inefficiency: minimal information is utilized in visual recognition tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 1449–1462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (1978). Knowledge structures and memory development. In R. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: what develops? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V., & Garnica, O. K. (1974). Is he coming or going? On the acquisition of deictic verbs. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 559–572.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cocchini, G., Logie, R. H., Della Sala, S., MacPherson, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (2002). Concurrent performance of two memory tasks: evidence for domain-specific working memory systems. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1086–1095.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, R. (1964). Acoustic confusion in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 75–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Courage, M. L., & Cowan, N. (Eds.). (2009). The development of memory in infancy and childhood. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 163–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (1992). Verbal memory span and the timing of spoken recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 668–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: an integrated framework. Oxford Psychology Series, no. 26. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 62–101). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. Hove, East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: how is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 51–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2011). The focus of attention as observed in visual working memory tasks: making sense of competing claims. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1401–1406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., AuBuchon, A. M., Gilchrist, A. L., Ricker, T. J., & Saults, J. S. (2011a). Age differences in visual working memory capacity: not based on encoding limitations. Developmental Science, 14, 1066–1074.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., Donnell, K., & Saults, J. S. (2013). A list-length constraint on incidental item-to-item associations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0447-7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., Li, D., Moffitt, A., Becker, T. M., Martin, E. A., Saults, J. S., et al. (2011b). A neural region of abstract working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 2852–2863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., & Morey, C. C. (2007). How can dual-task working memory retention limits be investigated? Psychological Science, 18, 686–688.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., Morey, C. C., AuBuchon, A. M., Zwilling, C. E., & Gilchrist, A. L. (2010). Seven-year-olds allocate attention like adults unless working memory is overloaded. Developmental Science, 13, 120–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., Nugent, L. D., Elliott, E. M., Ponomarev, I., & Saults, J. S. (1999). The role of attention in the development of short-term memory: age differences in the verbal span of apprehension. Child Development, 70, 1082–1097.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., Rouder, J. N., Blume, C. L., & Saults, J. S. (2012). Models of verbal working memory capacity: what does it take to make them work? Psychological Review, 119, 480–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N., Saults, J. S., Winterowd, C., & Sherk, M. (1991). Enhancement of 4-year-old children’s memory span for phonologically similar and dissimilar word lists. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 30–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Watkins, M. J. (1973). The role of rehearsal in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 599–607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333, 959–964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebbinghaus, H. (1885/1913). Memory: a contribution to experimental psychology. Translated by H.A. Ruger & C.E. Bussenius. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University (originally in German, Ueber das gedächtnis: Untersuchen zur experimentellen psychologie)

  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Chase, W. G., & Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of a memory skill. Science, 208, 1181–1182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Delaney, P. F., Weaver, G., & Mahadevan, R. (2004). Uncovering the structure of a memorist’s superior basic-memory capacity. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 191–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working-memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrell, S., & Lewandowsky, S. (2002). An endogenous distributed model of ordering in serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 59–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., Beach, D. H., & Chinsky, J. M. (1966). Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory task as a function of age. Child Development, 37, 283–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fougnie, D., & Marois, R. (2011). What limits working memory capacity? Evidence for modality-specific sources to the simultaneous storage of visual and auditory arrays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1329–1341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, L., Nussbaum, M., & Preiss, D. D. (2011). Is the use of information and communication technology related to performance in working memory tasks? Evidence from seventh-grade students. Computers & Education, 57, 2068–2076.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrity, L. I. (1975). An electromyographical study of subvocal speech and recall in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 11, 274–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: a longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Lamont, E., & Alloway, T. P. (2006). Working memory in the classroom. In S. J. Pickering (Ed.), Working memory and education (pp. 219–240). San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2008). An evolutionarily informed education science. Educational Psychologist, 43, 179–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R. (1982). Accessing one‐to‐one correspondence: still another paper about conservation. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 209–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilchrist, A. L., Cowan, N., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2009). Investigating the childhood development of working memory using sentences: new evidence for the growth of chunk capacity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 252–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G. S., Cowan, N., & Andrews, G. (2007). Separating cognitive capacity from knowledge: a new hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 236–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803–865.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, V. C. (2003). Educational psychology from 1890 to 1920. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Educational psychology: a century of contributions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains: dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 53, 449–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, D. F., Millis, K., Graesser, A. C., Butler, H., Forsyth, C., & Cai, Z. (2012). Operation ARA: a computerized learning game that teaches critical thinking and scientific reasoning. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7, 93–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). Organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henry, L. A. (1991). The effects of word length and phonemic similarity in young children’s short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43A, 35–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henson, R. N. A. (1998). Short-term memory for serial order: the start–end model. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 73–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., & Tordoff, V. (1989). Working memory development: the effects of speech rate, word length, and acoustic similarity on serial recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 72–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaeggi, S. M., & Buschkuehl, M. (2013). Training working memory. In T. P. Alloway & R. G. Alloway (Eds.), Working memory: the connected intelligence. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., & Citroën, R. (2013). Reevaluating key evidence for the development of rehearsal: phonological similarity effects in children are subject to proportional scaling artifacts. Developmental Psychology, 49, 837–847.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jevons, W. S. (1871). The power of numerical discrimination. Nature, 3, 281–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jolicoeur, P., & Dell’Acqua, R. (1998). The demonstration of short-term consolidation. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 138–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1990). The development of memory in children (3rd ed.). New York: W.H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86, 199–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: an experience–sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychological Science, 18, 614–621.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., Wilhelm, O., Payne, T. W., & Engle, R. W. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: a latent variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 189–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 317–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelligence, 14, 389–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kyndt, E., Cascallar, E., & Dochy, F. (2012). Individual differences in working memory capacity and attention, and their relationship with students’ approaches to learning. Higher Education: The International Journal of Higher Education and Educational Planning, 64, 285–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewandowsky, S., Duncan, M., & Brown, G. D. A. (2004). Time does not cause forgetting in short-term serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 771–790.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J. (1690). An essay concerning human understanding. London: Thomas Bassett.

  • Logie, R. H. (1996). The seven ages of working memory. In J. T. E. Richardson, R. W. Engle, L. Hasher, R. H. Logie, E. R. Stoltzfus, & R. T. Zacks (Eds.), Working memory and human cognition (pp. 31–65). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H., & Van Der Meulen, M. (2009). Fragmenting and integrating visuospatial working memory. In J. R. Brockmole (Ed.), Representing the visual world in memory (pp. 1–32). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390, 279–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., & Shebo, B. J. (1982). Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49, 270–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morey, C. C., & Bieler, M. (2013). Visual short-term memory always requires attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 163–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B. (1983). A distributed memory model for serial-order information. Psychological Review, 90, 316–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B., & Walker, K. D. (1969). Modality effects in free recall. Journal of Verbal Leaning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 665–676.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. J. (1974). Variations in children’s concepts by age and category. Child Development, 45, 577–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1968). Toward a theory of memory and attention. Psychological Review, 75(6), 522–536.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oberauer, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2008). Forgetting in immediate serial recall: decay, temporal distinctiveness, or interference? Psychological Review, 115, 544–576.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oberauer, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2011). Modeling working memory: a computational implementation of the time-based resource-sharing theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 10–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oberauer, K., Lewandowsky, S., Farrell, S., Jarrold, C., & Greaves, M. (2012). Modeling working memory: an interference model of complex span. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 779–819.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornstein, P. A., & Naus, M. J. (1978). Rehearsal processes in children’s memory. In P. A. Ornstein (Ed.), Memory development in children (pp. 69–99). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., & Sweller, J. (2012). An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 27–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J., & Johnson, J. (2011). A developmental theory of mental attention: its applications to measurement and task analysis. In P. Barrouillet & V. Gaillard (Eds.), Cognitive development and working memory: from neoPiagetian to cognitive approaches (pp. 13–46). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J., & Smith, J. (1969). The encoding and decoding of symbols by children: a new experimental paradigm and a neo-Piagetian model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8, 328–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penney, C. G. (1989). Modality effects and the structure of short-term verbal memory. Memory & Cognition, 17, 398–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, S. J. (2006). Working memory and education. San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 359–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricker, T. J., & Cowan, N. (2010). Loss of visual working memory within seconds: the combined use of refreshable and non-refreshable features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1355–1368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricker, T.J., & Cowan, N. (2013). Differences between presentation methods in working memory procedures: a matter of working memory consolidation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. doi:10.1037/a0034301.

  • Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Cowan, N., Zwilling, C. E., Morey, C. C., & Pratte, M. S. (2008). An assessment of fixed-capacity models of visual working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), 105, 5975–5979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabol, T. J., & Pianta, R. C. (2012). Patterns of school readiness forecast achievement and socioemotional development at the end of elementary school. Child Development, 83, 282–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saltz, E., Soller, E., & Sigel, I. E. (1972). The development of natural language concepts. Child Development, 1972(43), 1191–1202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, A., Scheiter, K., & Genuchten, E. (2011). The role of working memory in multimedia instruction: is working memory working during learning from text and pictures? Educational Psychology Review, 23, 389–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shim, W., & Walczak, K. (2012). The impact of faculty teaching practices on the development of students’ critical thinking skills. International Journal of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education, 24, 16–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138, 628–654.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slevc, L. (2011). Saying what’s on your mind: working memory effects on sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1503–1514.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Toplak, M. E. (2013). Myside bias, rational thinking, and intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 259–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Süß, H. M., Oberauer, K., Wittmann, W. W., Wilhelm, O., & Schulze, R. (2002). Working-memory capacity explains reasoning ability—and a little bit more. Intelligence, 30, 261–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2011). Cognitive load theory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 55, 37–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. G. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tan, L., & Ward, G. (2000). A recency-based account of the primacy effect in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1589–1625.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, J. J., & Marois, R. (2004). Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature, 428, 751–754.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 242–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tricot, A., & Sweller, J. (2013). Domain-specific knowledge and why teaching generic skills does not work. Educational Psychology Review. doi:10.1007/s10648-013-9243-1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Der Meulen, M., Logie, R. H., & Sala, S. D. (2009). Selective interference with image retention and generation: evidence for the workspace model. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1568–1580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vergauwe, E., Barrouillet, P., & Camos, V. (2010). Do mental processes share a domain-general resource? Psychological Science, 21, 384–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, E. K., McCollough, A. W., & Machizawa, M. G. (2005). Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature, 438, 500–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C., & Shapiro, D. (1971). Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning problem. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 63–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilding, J. (2001). Over the top: are there exceptions to the basic capacity limit? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 152–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (Eds.). (2005). Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1894/1998). Lectures on human and animal psychology. Translated from the second German edition by J.E. Creighton & E.B. Titchener. Bristol, UK: Thoemmes Press.

  • Xu, Y., & Chun, M. M. (2006). Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects. Nature, 440, 91–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, W., & Luck, S. J. (2009). Sudden death and gradual decay in visual working memory. Psychological Science, 20, 423–428.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was completed with support from NIH grant R01-HD21338.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nelson Cowan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cowan, N. Working Memory Underpins Cognitive Development, Learning, and Education. Educ Psychol Rev 26, 197–223 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-013-9246-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-013-9246-y

Keywords

Navigation