Skip to main content
Log in

Metacognitive skills and intellectual ability of young adolescents: a longitudinal study from a developmental perspective

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the last decades, students increasingly have been placed in the role of active learners with responsibilities for their own learning. Students have to be able to plan their learning activities and execute them in a systematic and orderly way and to monitor and to evaluate their learning and to reflect on it. All aforementioned skills are components of metacognitive skillfulness. The first objective was to gain insight in the development of both quantity and quality of metacognitive skills in young adolescents (aged 12–15 years). The second objective was to establish whether development of metacognitive skills is intelligence related or relatively intelligence independent. Finally, the generality vs. domain specificity of developing metacognitive skills was investigated. In a 3-year longitudinal study, participants performed two different tasks (text studying and problem solving) in two different domains (history and math), while thinking aloud. Results show that between the age of 12 and 15 years, metacognitive skills do not develop linearly or at the same pace. Furthermore, metacognitive skills contribute to learning performance, partly independent of intellectual ability. Finally, the results show that metacognitive skills appear to be predominantly general by nature over the years. Although a smaller domain-specific component was found as well in the first 2 years, this component disintegrated in the third year. The age around 15 years appears to be a relevant point in time during the developmental trajectory of metacognitive skills: Growth is (temporarily) put on hold, while the nature of these skills becomes fully general.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Effects of selective loss of participants over the years were checked for and not found. The same applies for the effect of gender.

  2. Mean p values for history over the years were .51, .54, and .56, respectively. Mean p values for math over the years were .44, .69, and .66, respectively. ANOVA and post hoc test (Bonferroni) on the math data revealed no significant differences between years.

References

  • Alexander, J., Carr, M., & Schwanenflugel, M. (1995). Development of metacognition in gifted children: directions for future research. Developmental Review, 15, 1–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1996). The architecture of cognition. Mohwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Annevirta, T., & Vauras, M. (2006). Developmental changes of metacognitive skill in elementary school children. The Journal of Experimental Education, 74, 197–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, J. E. (2000). Self-regulated reading and test preparation among college students. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 31, 42–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blöte, A. W., Otterloo, S. G., Van Stevenson, C. E., & Veenman, M. V. J. (2004). Discovery and maintenance of the many-to-one counting strategy in 4-year-olds: a microgenetic study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 83–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borkowski, J. G., & Peck, V. A. (1986). Causes and consequences of metamemory in gifted children. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), Knowing, learning, and instruction. Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 393–451). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowen, S., Shore, B. M., & Cartwright, G. F. (1992). Do gifted children use computers differently? A view from “the factory”. Gifted Educational International, 8, 151–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L. (1980). Metacognitive development and reading. In R. J. Spiro, B. Bruce, & W. F. Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L., & DeLoache, J. S. (1978). Skills, plans, and self-regulation. In R. S. Siegel (Ed.), Children’s thinking: what develops? Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L., Brandsford, J. D., Ferrara, R. A., & Campione, J. C. (1983). Learning, remembering, and understanding. In J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (4th ed). Cognitive development (Vol. 3, pp. 515–529). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buskop, H., Dalhuisen, L., & Van Der Geest, R. (1998). Sprekend verleden, deel 2. Baarn: NijghVersluys.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities. A survey of factor-analytic studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28, 62–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christoph, N. L. H. (2006). The role of metacognitive skills in learning to solve problems. Wageningen: Prinsen & Looijen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crone, E. A., Wendelken, C., Donohue, S., Van Leijenhorst, L., & Bunge, S. A. (2006). Neurocognitive development of the ability to manipulate information in working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 9315–9320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, F. P. C. M. (1992). Zelfstandig leren. Regulatie van het leerproces en leren reguleren: een procesbenadering. Tilburg: Katholieke Universiteit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demetriou, A., & Efklides, A. (1990). The objective and subjective structure of problem-solving abilities: metacognitive awareness from early adolescence to middle age. In H. Mandl, E. de Corte, S. N. Bennett, & H. F. Friedrich (Eds.), Learning and instruction in an international context: volume 2.1. Social and cognitive aspects of learning and instruction (pp. 161–179). Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1992). Perspectives on perspective taking. In H. Beilin & P. Pufall (Eds.), Piaget’s theory: prospects and possibilities (pp. 107–141). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, R., Schauble, L., Raghavan, K., & Zeitz, C. (1992). Scientific reasoning across different domains. In E. de Corte, M. C. Linn, H. Mandl, & L. Verschaffel (Eds.), Computer-based learning environments and problem solving. NATO ASI series F, vol. 84 (pp. 345–371). Heidelberg: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1965). Fundamental statistics in psychology and education. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. London: Routledge and Kegan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, W. L., Frost, P. J., & Weaver, C. A., III. (2000). Individual differences in metacognition: evidence against a general metacognitive ability. Memory and Cognition, 28, 92–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinnunen, R., & Vauras, M. (1995). Comprehension monitoring and the level of comprehension in high- and low-achieving primary school childrens’ reading. Learning and Instruction, 5, 143–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (1999). Metacognitive development. In L. Balter & C. S. Tamis-Lemonda (Eds.), Child psychology. A handbook of contemporary issues (pp. 259–286). Philadelphia: Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 178–181.

  • Larkin, S. (2006). Collaborative group work and individual development of metacognition in early years. Research in Science Education, 36, 7–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2006). Precursors of metamemory in young children: the role of theory of mind and metacognitive vocabulary. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 15–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. (1977). Realizing that you don’t understand: a preliminary investigation. Child Development, 48, 986–992.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. (1979). Realizing that you don’t understand: elementary school children’s awareness of inconsistencies. Child Development, 50, 643–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meijer, J., Veenman, M. V. J., & Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M. (2006). Metacognitive activities in text studying and problem solving: development of a taxonomy. Educational Research and Evaluation, 3, 209–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mevarech, Z., & Fridkin, S. (2006). The effects of IMPROVE on mathematical knowledge, mathematical reasoning and metacognition. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 85–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R., Perkins, D., & Smith, E. (1985). The teaching of thinking. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. C. (1967). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pedhazur, E. J. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research (2nd ed.). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M. (2000). Development of grounded theories of complex cognitive processing: exhaustive within- and between-study analyses of think-aloud data. In G. Schraw & J. C. Impara (Eds.), Issues in the measurement of metacognition. Lincoln: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: the nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., Yokoi, L., Van Meter, P., Van Etten, S., & Freebern, G. (1997). Some of the reasons why preparing for an exam is so hard: what can be done to make it easier? Educational Psychology Review, 9, 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prins, F. J., Veenman, M. V. J., & Elshout, J. J. (2006). The impact of intellectual ability and metacognition on learning: new support for the threshold of problematicity theory. Learning and Instruction, 4, 374–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G., & Perkins, D. N. (1989). Rocky roads to transfer: rethinking mechanisms of a neglected phenomenon. Educational Psychologist, 24, 113–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (1985). Developmental trends in the meta-memory memory behavior relationship: An integrative review. In D. L. Forrest-Pressley, G. E. Mac Kinnon, & T. G. Wallers (Eds.), Metacognition, cognition and human performance (Vol. 1, pp. 57–109). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1997). Memory development between two and twenty (2nd ed.). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: problem solving, metacognition, and sense-making in mathematics. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook for research on mathematics and learning (pp. 334–370). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., & Nietfeld, J. (1998). A further test of the general monitoring skill hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 236–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., Dunkle, M. E., Bendixen, L. D., & Roedel, T. D. (1995). Does a general monitoring skill exist? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 433–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shore, B. M., & Lazar, L. (1996). IQ-related differences in time allocation during problem solving. Psychological Reports, 78, 848–849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., DeLoache, J. S., & Eisenberg, N. (2010). How children develop (3rd ed.). New York: Worth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slife, B. D., Weiss, J., & Bell, T. (1985). Separability of metacognition and cognition: problem solving in learning disabled and regular students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 437–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavior manifestations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 417–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 69–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Metaphors of the mind: conceptions of the nature of intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, K. J., & Satterly, D. J. H. (1990). The development and inter-relationship of metacognitive components among primary school children. Educational Psychology, 10, 5–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toga, A. W., Thompson, P. M., & Sowell, E. R. (2006). Mapping brain maturation. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 148–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Boxtel, C. A. M., & Schrover, W. (1998). MeMo: Geschiedenis voor de basisvorming, deel 2, Mavo/Havo/Vwo. Den Bosch: Malmberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Stel, M., & Veenman, M. V. J. (2008). Relation between intellectual ability and metacognitive skillfulness as predictors of learning performance of young students performing tasks in different domains. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 128–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Stel, M., & Veenman, M. V. J. (2010). Development of metacognitive skillfulness: a longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 220–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Stel, M., Veenman, M. V. J., Deelen, K., & Haenen, J. (2010). Increasing role of metacognitive skills in math: a cross-sectional study from a developmental perspective. ZDM International Journal on Mathematics Education, 42, 219–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk, H., & Tellegen, P. J. (1994). GIVO. Groninger Intelligentietest Voortgezet Onderwijs. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M. (1986). Markeren van kerngedeelten in studieteksten. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J. (1993). Intellectual ability and metacognitive skill: determinants of discovery learning in computerized environments. Dissertation, Amsterdam: Universiteit van Amsterdam.

  • Veenman, M. V. J. (2011). Learning to self-monitor and self-regulate. In R. Mayer & P. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of research on learning and instruction (pp. 197–218). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., & Beishuizen, J. J. (2004). Intellectual and metacognitive skills of novices while studying texts under conditions of text difficulty and time constraint. Learning and Instruction, 14, 621–640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., & Spaans, M. A. (2005). Relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills: age and task differences. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 159–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., & Verheij, J. (2003). Technical students’ metacognitive skills: relating general vs. specific metacognitive skills to study success. Learning and Individual Differences, 13, 259–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., Elshout, J. J., & Meijer, J. (1997). The generality vs. domain-specifity of metacognitive skills in novice learning across domains. Learning and Instruction, 7, 187–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., Kerseboom, L., & Imthorn, C. (2000). Test anxiety and metacognitive skillfulness: availability versus production deficiencies. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 13, 391–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., Wilhelm, P., & Beishuizen, J. J. (2004). The relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills from a developmental perspective. Learning and Instruction, 14, 89–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 3–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., Bavelaar, L., De Wolf, L., & Van Haaren, M. G. P. (2013). The on-line assessment of metacognitive skills in a computerized learning environment. Learning and Individual Differences. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vuijk, R., et al. (2003). Getal en Ruimte. Houten: EPN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vukman, K. B., & Licardo, M. (2010). How cognitive, metacognitive, motivational and emotional self-regulation influence school performance in adolescence and early adulthood. Educational Studies, 36, 259–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M. C., Haertel, G. D., & Walberg, H. J. (1990). What influences learning? A content analysis of review literature. The Journal of Educational Research, 84, 30–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M. (1990). Children’s theories of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitebread, D., Coltman, P., Pino Pasternak, D., Sangster, C., Grau, V., Bingham, S., et al. (2009). The development of two observational tools for assessing metacognition and self-regulated learning in young children. Metacognition and Learning, 4, 63–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (1996). A metacognitive view of individual differences in self-regulated learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 327–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (2010). Improving measurements of self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 45, 267–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Manita van der Stel.

Additional information

Manita van der Stel. Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, 2333 AK Leiden, The Netherlands. Tel: +31-71-5615611, E-mail: mvdstel@planet.nl

Current themes of research:

Development of metacognitive skills and teaching metacognitive skills.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education:

Van der Stel, M., & Veenman, M. V. J. (2008). Relation between intellectual ability and metacognitive skillfulness as predictors of learning performance of young students performing tasks in different domains. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 128–134.

Van der Stel, M., & Veenman, M. V. J. (2010). Development of metacognitive skillfulness: A longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 220–224.

Van der Stel, M., Veenman, M. V. J., Deelen, K., & Haenen, J. (2010). Increasing role of metacognitive skills in math: A cross-sectional study from a developmental perspective. ZDM International Journal on Mathematics Education, 42, 219–229.

Marcel V. J. Veenman. Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, 2333 AK Leiden, The Netherlands. Tel: +31-71-5273463, E-mail: veenman@fsw.leidenuniv.nl

Current themes of research:

Metacognitive skills and giftedness; metacognitive skills and test anxiety.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education:

Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 3–14.

Veenman, M. V. J. (2008). Giftedness: Predicting the speed of expertise acquisition by intellectual ability and metacognitive skillfulness of novices. In: M. F. Shaughnessy, M. V. J (ed.).

Veenman, & C. Kleyn-Kennedy (Eds.), Meta-cognition: A recent review of research, theory, and perspectives (pp. 207–220). Hauppauge: Nova Science Publishers.

Veenman, M. V. J. (2011). Learning to self-monitor and self-regulate. In: R. Mayer, & P. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of research on learning and instruction (pp. 197–218). New York: Routledge.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

van der Stel, M., Veenman, M.V.J. Metacognitive skills and intellectual ability of young adolescents: a longitudinal study from a developmental perspective. Eur J Psychol Educ 29, 117–137 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-013-0190-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-013-0190-5

Keywords

Navigation