Skip to main content
Log in

Spatial cognition in the gerbil: computing optimal escape routes from visual threats

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous studies in our laboratory have shown that when presented with a sudden stimulus simulating an oncoming predator, Mongolian gerbils can compute the optimal trajectory to a safe refuge, taking into account the position of the threat, the location of a clearly visible refuge, and several other contextual variables as well. In the present studies, the main goal was to explore the abilities of gerbils to use mental representations of spaces that were visually occluded by opaque barriers to compute efficient escape trajectories. In all studies, gerbils were placed into a round open field containing a single refuge. On each trial, an overhead visual stimulus was caused to ‘fly’ overhead, eliciting robust escape movements from the gerbils. By manipulating the shape and position of a series of opaque barriers that were interposed between the gerbils and the refuge, we were able to show that gerbils can compute the shortest route to an invisible target, even when the available routes to the target are made complex by using elaborate barrier shapes. These findings suggest that gerbils can maintain representations of their locations with respect to salient environmental landmarks and refuges, even when such locations are not continuously visible.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ågren G (1976) Social and territorial behaviour in the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus) under seminatural conditions. Biol Behav 1:276–285

    Google Scholar 

  • Ågren G, Zhou Q, Zhong W (1989) Ecology and social behaviour of Mongolian gerbils, Meriones unguiculatus, at Xilinhot, Inner Mongolia, China. Anim Behav 37:11–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett AG (1996) Do animals have cognitive maps? J Exp Biol 199(1):219–224

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chapuis N, Scardigl P (1993) Shortcut ability in hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus): the role of environmental and kinesthetic information. Learn Behav 21(3):255–265

    Google Scholar 

  • Collett TS (1982) Do toads plan routes? A study of the detour behaviour of Bufo viridis. J Comp Physiol A 146(2):261–271

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collett TS, Harkness LIK (1982) Depth vision in animals. In: Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJW (eds) Analysis of visual behavior. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 111–176

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark MM, Galef BB (1979) A sensitive period for the maintenance of emotionality in Mongolian gerbils. J Comp Physiol Psychol 93:200–210

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eilam D, Golani I (1989) Home base behavior of rats (Rattus norvegicus) exploring a novel environment. Behav Brain Res 34(3):199–211

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ellard CG (1993) Organization of escape movements from overhead threats in the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). J Comp Psychol 107(3):242–249

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ellard C (1996) Laboratory studies of antipredator behavior in the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus): factors affecting response attenuation with repeated presentations. J Comp Psychol 110(2):155–163

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ellard CG, Goodale MA (1988) A functional analysis of the collicular output pathways: a dissociation of deficits following lesions of the dorsal tegmental decussation and the ipsilateral colliculus efferent bundle in the Mongolian gerbil. Exp Brain Res 71(2):307–319

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ellard CG (2008) You are here. Doubleday, New York (in press)

  • Ellard CG, Shankar S (2001) Distance computation and spatial navigation in the gerbil: a comparison of contributions from open and closed loop movement control. In: Chen L, Zhuo Y (eds) Proceedings of the third international conference on cognitive science, Press of University of Science and Technology of China, Beijing, pp 29–34

  • Fiset S, Doré FY (1996) Spatial encoding in domestic cats (Felis catus). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 22(4):420–437

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel CR (1993) The organization of learning. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodale MA (1983) Neural mechanisms of visual orientation in rodents: targets versus places. In: Hein A, Jeannerod M (eds) Spatially oriented behavior. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 35–62

    Google Scholar 

  • Hemmi JM (2004) Predator avoidance in fiddler crabs: 1. Escape decisions in relation to the risk of predation. Anim Behav 69(3):603–614

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Healey S (1998) Spatial representation in animals. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Howland HC (1974) Optimal strategies for predator avoidance: the relative importance of speed and manoevrability. J Theoret Biol 47:333–350

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer DL, Bonenfant M (1997) Direction of predator approach and the decision to flee to a refuge. Anim Behav 54:289–295

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Menzel R, Gregger U, Smith A, Berger S, Brandt R, Brunke S, Bundrock G, Hülse S, Plümpe T, Schaupp F, Schüttler E, Stach S, Stindt J, Stollhoff J, Watzl S (2005) Honeybees navigate according to a map-like spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102(8):3040–3045

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Müller M, Wehner R (1988) Path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 85(14):5287–5290

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rydell J, Skals N, Surlykke A, Svenson M (1997) Hearing and bat defence in geometrid winter moths. Proc R Soc Lond B 254:83–88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherry DF (1998) The ecology and neurobiology of spatial memory. In: Dukas R (ed) Cognitive ecology. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 261–296

    Google Scholar 

  • Tarsitano MS, Andrew R (1999) Scanning and route selection in the jumping spider Portia labiata. Anim Behav 58(2):255–265

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tchernichovski O, Benjamini Y, Golani I (1998) The dynamics of long-term exploration in the rat. Part I. A phase-plane analysis of the relationship between location and velocity. Biol Cybern 78(6):423–432

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thinus-Blanc C (1996) Animal spatial cognition: behavioural and brain approaches. World Scientific, Hackensack

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman EC (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol Rev 55:189–208

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wang RF, Spelke E (2002) Human spatial representation: insights from animals. Trends Cogn Sci 6(9):376–382

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ydenberg RC, Dill LM (1986) The economics of fleeing from predators. Adv Study Behav 16:229–249

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Colin G. Ellard.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ellard, C.G., Eller, M.C. Spatial cognition in the gerbil: computing optimal escape routes from visual threats. Anim Cogn 12, 333–345 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-008-0193-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-008-0193-9

Keywords

Navigation