Skip to main content
Log in

The effects of an unexpected spider stimulus on skin conductance responses and eye movements: an inattentional blindness study

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Are spiders evolutionarily relevant threat cues that capture attention automatically and preattentively—also in non-fearful persons? Previous studies concerning this question did not examine responses to unexpected spider stimuli, although this is an ecologically valid situation. Therefore, we conducted an inattentional blindness (IB) experiment. While unselected participants (N = 120) were focusing their attention on a visual discrimination task, suddenly a spider or a flower image appeared. The rates of reported detection were not enhanced for spider images, and fear of spiders had no moderating influence on IB rates. However, spider pictures in general elicited greater skin conductance responses than flower pictures, and this difference was also observed for pictures that were not reported afterward. Moreover, eye movement data revealed that the spider stimulus was initially fixated more often than the flower stimulus. In sum, our findings imply that enhanced reactions to unexpected spider images are independent of fear level and include early engagement processes. Awareness of a spider picture does not seem to be a prerequisite for elevated arousal.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In exploratory analyses, we also defined high and low fearful groups by means of a median split or by comparing the 25% with the lowest and the highest scores, respectively. These analyses, as with a continuous variable, did not reveal significant differences between high and low fearful participants in detection rates, skin conductance responses and eye movements.

  2. In a different analysis with a time window of 1–5 s after stimulus onset and a minimum response criterion of 0.01 µS, all significant results were maintained.

  3. The leaves of the flower used in the experiment share features with the leg of a spider. However, we avoided a flower stem without leaves because of shared features with the discrimination task’s cross; shared features are known to cause reduced IB effects (Mack & Rock, 1998).

References

  • Alpers, G. W., Gerdes, A. B. M., Lagarie, B., Tabbert, K., Vaitl, D., & Stark, R. (2009). Attention and amygdala activity: An fMRI study with spider pictures in spider phobia. Journal of Neural Transmission, 116, 747–757.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Batty, M. J., Cave, K. R., & Pauli, P. (2005). Abstract stimuli associated with threat through conditioning cannot be detected preattentively. Emotion, 5, 418–430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beanland, V., & Pammer, K. (2010). Looking without seeing or seeing without looking? Eye movements in sustained inattentional blindness. Vision Research, 50, 977–988.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C., & Damasio, A. R. (1995). Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269, 1115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Breska, A., Maoz, K., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (2011). Interstimulus intervals for skin conductance response measurement. Psychophysiology, 48, 437–440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Büchel, C., Morris, J., Dolan, R. J., & Friston, K. J. (1998). Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 20, 947–957.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlsson, K., Petersson, K. M., Lundqvist, D., Karlsson, A., Ingvar, M., & Öhman, A. (2004). Fear and the amygdala: Manipulation of awareness generates differential cerebral responses to phobic and fear-relevant (but non-feared) stimuli. Emotion, 4, 340–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cavanagh, K., & Davey, G. C. L. (2000). UCS expectancy biases in spider phobics: Underestimation of aversive consequences following fear-irrelevant stimuli. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 641–651.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681–700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerdes, A. B. M., Alpers, G. W., & Pauli, P. (2008). When spiders appear suddenly: Spider-phobic patients are distracted by task-irrelevant spiders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 174–187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerdes, A. B. M., Pauli, P., & Alpers, G. W. (2009). Toward and away from spiders: Eye-movements in spider-fearful participants. Journal of Neural Transmission, 116, 725–733.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamm, A. O., & Weike, A. I. (2005). The neuropsychology of fear learning and fear regulation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57, 5–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 284–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchner, H., & Thorpe, S. J. (2006). Ultra-rapid object detection with saccadic eye movements: Visual processing speed revisited. Vision Research, 46, 1762–1776.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klucken, T., Kagerer, S., Schweckendiek, J., Tabbert, K., Vaitl, D., & Stark, R. (2009). Neural, electrodermal and behavioral response patterns in contingency aware and unaware subjects during a picture–picture conditioning paradigm. Neuroscience, 158, 721–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kolassa, I.-T., Buchmann, A., Lauche, R., Kolassa, S., Partchev, I., Miltner, W., et al. (2007). Spider phobics more easily see a spider in morphed schematic pictures. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3, 59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., van Damme, S., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004a). Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotion, 4, 312–317.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004b). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1183–1192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155–184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, H., & Telch, M. J. (2008). Attentional biases in social anxiety: An investigation using the inattentional blindness paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 819–835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lipp, O. V., & Derakshan, N. (2005). Attentional bias to pictures of fear-relevant animals in a dot probe task. Emotion, 5, 365–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lipp, O. V., & Waters, A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background: Attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distractors is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion, 7, 192–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1985). Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 563–569.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, B., Muris, P., Vogel, L., Nojoredjo, I., & Merckelbach, H. (2006). Fear-relevant change detection in spider-fearful and non-fearful participants. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20, 510–519.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miltner, W. H. R., Krieschel, S., Hecht, H., Trippe, R., & Weiss, T. (2004). Eye movements and behavioral responses to threatening and nonthreatening stimuli during visual search in phobic and nonphobic subjects. Emotion, 4, 323–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nummenmaa, L., Hyönä, J., & Calvo, M. G. (2006). Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. Emotion, 6, 257–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., Carlsson, K., Lundqvist, D., & Ingvar, M. (2007). On the unconscious subcortical origin of human fear. Physiology & Behavior, 92, 180–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130, 466–478.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1994). “Unconscious anxiety”: Phobic responses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 231–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L., Japee, S., Sturman, D., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2006). Target visibility and visual awareness modulate amygdala responses to fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 366–375.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., & Morland, T. (2005). Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. Neuroimage, 28, 249–255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phaf, R. H., & Kan, K. J. (2007). The automaticity of emotional Stroop: A meta-analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38(2), 184–199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pischek-Simpson, L. K., Boschen, M. J., Neumann, D. L., & Waters, A. M. (2009). The development of an attentional bias for angry faces following Pavlovian fear conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 322–330.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Recio, G., Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2009). Effects of inter-stimulus interval on skin conductance responses and event-related potentials in a Go/NoGo task. Biological Psychology, 80, 246–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rinck, M., Bundschuh, S., Englert, S., Müller, A., Wissmann, J., Ellwart, T., et al. (2002). Reliabilität und Validität dreier Instrumente zur Messung von Angst vor Spinnen. Diagnostica, 48, 141–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinck, M., Reinecke, A., Ellwart, T., Heuer, K., & Becker, E. S. (2005). Speeded detection and increased distraction in fear of spiders: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 235–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Straube, T., Mentzel, H. J., & Miltner, W. H. (2006). Neural mechanisms of automatic and direct processing of phobogenic stimuli in specific phobia. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 162–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Szczepanowski, R., & Pessoa, L. (2007). Fear perception: Can objective and subjective awareness measures be dissociated? Journal of Vision, 7, 10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Usunoff, K. G., Schmitt, O., Itzev, D. E., Rolfs, A., & Wree, A. (2007). Efferent connections of the parabigeminal nucleus to the amygdala and the superior colliculus in the rat: A double-labeling fluorescent retrograde tracing study. Brain Research, 1133, 87–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Várkuti, B., & Ambach, W. (2008). Bootstrapping und andere Standardisierungsverfahren: Was ist wann am besten? http://www.igpp.de/english/es/pdf/TeaP_2008_Varkuti.pdf.

  • Watts, F. N., McKenna, F. P., Sharrock, R., & Trezise, L. (1986). Colour naming of phobia-related words. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 97–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wieser, M. J., Pauli, P., & Mühlberger, A. (2009). Probing the attentional control theory in social anxiety: An emotional saccade task. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 314–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1999). Inattentional amnesia. In V. Coltheart (Ed.), Fleeting memories (pp. 71–94). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wurtz, R. H., & Goldberg, M. E. (1971). Superior colliculus cell responses related to eye movements in awake monkeys. Science, 171, 82–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 665–681.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Roswitha Gerhard and Silja Menken for their help. This work was supported by the DFG (German Research Foundation), Research Group ‘Emotion and Behavior’ (FOR 605) and Collaborative Research Center “fear, anxiety, anxiety disorders (SFB-TRR 58, project B01)”.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. We have full control of all primary data and agree to allow the journal to review our data if requested.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Pauli.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wiemer, J., Gerdes, A.B.M. & Pauli, P. The effects of an unexpected spider stimulus on skin conductance responses and eye movements: an inattentional blindness study. Psychological Research 77, 155–166 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0407-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0407-7

Keywords

Navigation