Skip to main content
Log in

Left of centre: asymmetries for the horizontal vertical line illusion

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study explored the mechanisms that underlie asymmetries for the horizontal vertical illusion (HVI), which deceives length perception, so that a vertical line is perceived as longer than a horizontal line of equivalent length. In Experiment 1, university students (n = 14) made length judgements for vertical and horizontal lines. The vertical line was shifted in eight steps from the far left of the horizontal line (⌊) to the far right (⌋). An HVI was observed for the medial positions (⊥), which diminished towards the lateral positions. The HVI was also stronger when the vertical line was on the left. Because the left/right asymmetry changed as a function of lateral/medial position, the asymmetry within the HVI stimulus is most likely the result of pseudoneglect, which affects judgements of horizontal length. In Experiment 2, participants (n = 15) made judgements for HVI stimuli presented to the left- and right-hemispace and the midline. The HVI was stronger in the left hemispace. Because the asymmetry between the left- and right-hemispaces did not interact with the asymmetry within the stimuli, it was concluded that the asymmetry between hemispatial positions was the result of right hemisphere susceptibility to illusory geometrical effects whereas the asymmetry within the stimulus is related to an object-centred attentional asymmetry. The HVI is affected by asymmetries in length judgements and susceptibility to illusions and may provide interesting insights into attentional disorders in clinical populations, such as neglect.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Armstrong, L., & Marks, L. E. (1997). Differential effects of stimulus context on perceived length: implications for the horizontal-vertical illusion. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1200–1213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjoertomt, O., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2002). Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain, 125, 2012–2022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charles, J., Sahraie, A., & McGeorge, P. (2007). Hemispatial asymmetries in judgment of stimulus size. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 687–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charras, P., & Lupiáñez, J. (2009). The relevance of symmetry in line length perception. Perception, 38, 1428–1438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charras, P., & Lupiáñez, J. (2010). Length perception of horizontal and vertical bisected lines. Psychological Research, 74, 196–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clem, R. K., & Pollack, R. H. (1975). Illusion magnitude as a function of visual field exposure. Perception & Psychophysics, 17, 450–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Miezin, F. M., Shulman, G. L., & Petersen, S. E. (1993). A PET study of visuospatial attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1202–1226.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daini, R., Angelelli, P., Antonucci, G., Cappa, S. F., & Vallar, G. (2002). Exploring the syndrome of spatial unilateral neglect through an illusion of length. Experimental Brain Research, 144, 224–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J., & Halligan, P. W. (1991). Can visual neglect operate in object-centered co-ordinates? An affirmative single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 475–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finger, F. W., & Spelt, D. K. (1947). The illustration of the horizontal-vertical illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, 243–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foxe, J. F., McCourt, M. E., & Javitt, D. C. (2003). Right hemisphere control of visuospatial attention: Line-bisection judgments evaluated with high-density electrical mapping and source analysis. NeuroImage, 19, 710–726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grabowska, A., Szymanska, O., Nowicka, A., & Kwiecien, M. (1992). The effect of unilateral brain lesions on perception of visual illusions. Behavioural Brain Research, 47, 191–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, R. L. (1998). Eye and Brain, The Psychology of Seeing (5th ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hausmann, M. (2005). Hemispheric asymmetry in spatial attention across the menstrual cycle. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1559–1567.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., Watson, R. T., & Valenstein, E. (1993). Neglect and related disorders. In K. M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 279–336). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jewell, G., & McCourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, 93–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kubi, E., & Slotnick, B. M. (1993). The horizontal-vertical illusion: transfer of illusion decrement. Perceptual Motor Skills, 77, 339–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunnapas, T. M. (1955). An analysis of the ‘vertical-horizontal illusion’. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 134–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindell, A. K., & Nicholls, M. E. R. (2003). Cortical representation of the fovea: implications for visual half-field research. Cortex, 39, 111–117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, A. M., Nicholls, M. E. R., Mattingley, J. B., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2008). Left to right:representational biases for numbers and the effect of visuomotor adaptation. Cognition, 107, 1048–1058.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, A. M., Vijayakumar, N., & Nicholls, M. E. R. (2009). Prism adaptation overcomes pseudoneglect for the greyscales task. Cortex, 45, 537–543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattingley, J. B., Bradshaw, J. L., & Bradshaw, J. A. (1995). The effects of unilateral visuospatial neglect on perception of Müller-Lyer illusory figures. Perception & Psychophysics, 24, 415–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCourt, M. E. (2001). Performance consistency of normal observers in forced-choice tachistoscopic visual line bisection. Neuropsychologia, 39, 1065–1076.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCourt, M. E., & Jewell, G. (1999). Visuospatial attention in line bisection: stimulus modulation of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 37, 843–855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCourt, M. E., Garlinghouse, M., & Slater, J. (2000). Centripetal versus centrifugal bias in visual line bisection: focusing attention on two hypotheses. Frontiers in Bioscience, 5, d58–d71.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., & Roberts, G. R. (2002). Can free-viewing perceptual asymmetries be explained by scanning, pre-motor or attentional biases? Cortex, 38, 113–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., Hughes, G., Mattingley, J. B., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2004). Are object and space-based attentional biases both important to free-viewing perceptual asymmetries? Experimental Brain Research, 154, 513–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ota, H., Fujii, T., Suzuki, K., Fukatsu, R., & Yamadori, A. (2001). Dissociation of body-centred and stimulus-centred representations in unilateral neglect. Neurology, 57, 2064–2069.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porac, C., Coren, S., Girgus, J. S., & Verde, M. (1979). Visual-geometric illusions: unisex phenomena. Perception, 8, 401–412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Post, R. B., Caufield, K. J., & Welch, R. B. (2001). Contributions of object- and space-based mechanisms to line bisection errors. Neuropsychologia, 39, 856–864.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prinzmetal, W., & Gettleman, L. (1993). Vertical-horizontal illusion: one eye is better than two. Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 81–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmjou, S., Hausmann, M., & Güntürkün, O. (1999). Hemispheric dominance and gender in the perception of an illusion. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1041–1047.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Kinsbourne, M., & Moscovitch, M. (1990). Hemispheric control of spatial attention. Brain and Cognition, 12, 240–266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, H. O., Wennberg, P., & Raudsepp, J. (2007). The effects of inverting prisms on the horizontal–vertical illusion: a systematic effect of downward gaze. Experimental Brain Research, 183, 9–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ro, T., & Rafal, R. D. (1996). Perception of geometric illusions in hemispatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 34, 973–978.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothwell, B., & Zaidel, E. (1990). Visual field differences in the magnitude of the Opel-Kundt illusion vary with processing time. Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 180–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., & Behrmann, M. (1996). Object-centred not scene-based visual neglect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1261–1278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waberski, T. D., Gobbelé, R., Lamberty, K., Buchner, H., Marshall, J. C., & Fink, G. R. (2008). Timing of visuo-spatial information processing: electrical source imaging related to line bisection judgements. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1201–1210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weidner, R., & Fink, G. R. (2007). The neural mechanisms underlying the Müller-Lyer illusion and its interaction with visuospatial judgments. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 878–884.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, U., Maloney, L. T., & Tam, M. (2005). Distortions of perceived length in the frontoparallel plane: tests of perspective theories. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 967–979.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, T. L., Dixon, M. W., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Seeing big things: overestimation of heights is greater for real objects than for objects in pictures. Perception, 28, 445–467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge Mark McCourt and another anonymous reviewer for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. Funding for this research was provided by an Australian Research Council grant (DP 0986118).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael E. R. Nicholls.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Josev, E.K., Forte, J.D. & Nicholls, M.E.R. Left of centre: asymmetries for the horizontal vertical line illusion. Psychological Research 75, 435–443 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0315-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0315-2

Keywords

Navigation