Skip to main content
Log in

Darwin’s “tug-of-war” vs. starlings’ “horse-racing”: how adaptations for sequential encounters drive simultaneous choice

  • Review
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Charles Darwin aided his private decision making by an explicit deliberation, famously deciding whether or not to marry by creating a list of points in a table with two columns: “Marry” and “Not Marry”. One hundred seventy-two years after Darwin’s wedding, we reconsider whether this process of choice, under which individuals assign values to their options and compare their relative merits at the time of choosing (the tug-of-war model), applies to our experimental animal, the European Starling, Sturnus vulgaris. We contrast this with the sequential choice model that postulates that decision-makers make no comparison between options at the time of choice. According to the latter, behaviour in simultaneous choices reflects adaptations to contexts with sequential encounters, in which the choice is whether to take an opportunity or let it pass. We postulate that, in sequential encounters, the decision-maker assigns (by learning) a subjective value to each option, reflecting its payoff relative to background opportunities. This value is expressed as latency and/or probability to accept each opportunity as opposed to keep searching. In simultaneous encounters, choice occurs through each option being processed independently, by a race between the mechanisms that generate option-specific latencies. We describe these alternative models and review data supporting the predictions of the sequential choice model.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aw J (2008) Decisions under uncertainty: common processes in birds, fish and humans. University of Oxford, Dissertation

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakker TCM, Milinski M (1991) Sequential female choice and the previous male effect in sticklebacks. Behav Ecol 29:205–210

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson M, Kacelnik A (1995) Preferences for fixed and variable food sources: variability in amount and delay. J Exp Anal Behav 63(3):313–329

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson M, Kacelnik A (1996) Rate currencies and the foraging starling: the fallacy of the averages revisited. Behav Ecol 7(3):341–352

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogacz R, Brown E, Moehlis J, Holmes P, Cohen JD (2006) The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. Psychol Rev 113(4):700–165

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury JW, Gibson RM (1983) Leks and mate choice. In: Bateson P (ed) Mate choice. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 109–138

    Google Scholar 

  • Catchpole CK, Dittami J, Leisler B (1984) Differential responses to male song repertoires in female songbirds implanted with oestradiol. Nature 312:563–564

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charnov EL (1976a) Optimal foraging: attack strategy of a mantid. Am Nat 110:145–156

    Google Scholar 

  • Charnov EL (1976b) Optimal foraging: the marginal value theorem. Theor Pop Biol 9:129–136

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin CR (1838) ‘This is the question Marry Not Marry’ [Memorandum on marriage]. CUL-DAR210.8.2 (Darwin Online, http://darwin-online.org.uk/)

  • Fantino E, Abarca N (1985) Choice, optimal foraging, and the delay-reduction hypothesis. Behav Brain Sci 8:315–329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin B (1772) Letter to Joseph Priestley. Printed 1987 in Writings, pp. 877–878. New York: The Library of America.

  • Freidin E, Aw J, Kacelnik A (2009) Sequential and simultaneous choices: testing the diet selection and sequential choice models. Behav Process 80:218–223

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson RM (1990) Relationship between blood parasites, mating success and phenotypic cues in male sage grouse Centrocercus urophasianus. Am Zool 30:271–278

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson RM, Langen TA (1996) How do animals choose their mates? Trends Ecol Evol 11:468–470

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes C (1998) Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behav Brain Sci 21:101–148

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hick WE (1952) On the rate of gain of information. Quart J Exp Psychol 4:11–26

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houston A (2010) Central-place foraging by humans: transport and processing. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. doi:10.1007/s00265-010-1119-5

  • Hyman R (1953) Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. J Exp Psychol 45:188–196

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Janetos AC (1980) Strategies of female mate choice: a theoretical analysis. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7:107–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik A (2003) The evolution of patience. In: Loewenstein G, Read D, Baumeister R (eds) Time and decision: economic and psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice. New York, Russell Sage, pp 115–138

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs JR, Kacelnik TP (1978) Tests of optimal sampling by foraging great tits. Nature 275:27–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh B, Schuck-Paim C, Kacelnik A (2004) Energetic state during learning affects foraging choices in starlings. Behav Ecol 15(3):396–399

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall J, McNamara J, Houston A (2010) The state of Darwinian theory. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. doi:10.1007/s00265-010-1121-y

  • Parker GA (1983) Mate quality and mating decisions. In: Bateson P (ed) Mate choice. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 141–166

    Google Scholar 

  • Pompilio L, Kacelnik A (2010) Context-dependent utility overrides absolute memory as a determinant of choice. PNAS 107(1):508–512

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pompilio L, Kacelnik A, Behmer ST (2006) State-dependent learned valuation drives choice in an invertebrate. Science 311:1613–1615

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Prokasy WF, Gorezano I (1979) The effect of US omission in classical aversive and appetitive conditioning of rabbits. Anim Learn Behav 7(1):80–88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Real L (1990) Search theory and mate choice. I. Models of single-sex discrimination. Am Nat 136:376–405

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Real L (1991) Search theory and mate choice. II. Mutual interaction, assortative mating, and equilibrium variation in male and female fitness. Am Nat 138:901–917

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reboreda JC, Kacelnik A (1991) Risk sensitivity in starlings: variability in food amount and food delay. Behav Ecol 2(4):301–308

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ (1985) The Túngara frog, a study in sexual selection and communication. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ, Wilczinski W (1988) Coevolution of sender and receiver: effect on local mate preference in cricket frogs. Science 240:1786–1788

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz L (2004) The paradox of choice: why more is less. Harper Collins, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro MS, Siller S, Kacelnik A (2008) Simultaneous and sequential choice as a function of reward delay and magnitude: normative, descriptive and process-based models tested in European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). J Exp Psych Anim Behav Proc 34:75–93

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic P (1995) The construction of preference. Am Psychol 50:364–371

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephens DW, Krebs JR (1986) Foraging theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Vasconcelos M, Urcuioli P (2008) Deprivation level and choice in pigeons: a test of within-trial contrast. Learn Behav 36:12–18

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vasconcelos M, Monteiro T, Aw J, Kacelnik A (2010) Choice in multi-alternative environments: a trial-by-trial implementation of the sequential choice model. Behav Process 84:435–439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittenberger JF (1983) Tactics of mate choice. In: Bateson P (ed) Mate choice. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 435–447

    Google Scholar 

  • Zupko J (2006) J Buridan. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/buridan/. Accessed 18 February 2010.

Download references

Author note

This work was supported by Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council Grant BB/G007144/1 to AK. MV, TM, and JA were supported by an Intra-European Marie Curie Fellowship, a doctoral grant from the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology, and a Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council Fellowship, respectively. We are grateful to Miguel Rodriguez-Gironés for discussions and advice.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alex Kacelnik.

Additional information

Communicated by Guest Editor A. Houston

This contribution is part of the Special Issue “Mathematical Models in Ecology and Evolution: Darwin 200” (see Marshall et al. 2010).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kacelnik, A., Vasconcelos, M., Monteiro, T. et al. Darwin’s “tug-of-war” vs. starlings’ “horse-racing”: how adaptations for sequential encounters drive simultaneous choice. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 65, 547–558 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1101-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1101-2

Keywords

Navigation