Skip to main content
Log in

Investigating the online control of goal-directed actions to a tactile target on the body

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Movement corrections to somatosensory targets have been found to be shorter in latency and larger in magnitude than corrections to external visual targets. Somatosensory targets (e.g., body positions) can be identified using both tactile (i.e., skin receptors) and proprioceptive information (e.g., the sense of body position derived from sensory organs in the muscles and joints). Here, we investigated whether changes in tactile information alone, without changes in proprioception, can elicit shorter correction latencies and larger correction magnitudes than those to external visual targets. Participants made reaching movements to a myofilament touching the index finger of the non-reaching finger (i.e., a tactile target) and a light-emitting diode (i.e., visual target). In one-third of the trials, target perturbations occurred 100 ms after movement onset, such that the target was displaced 3 cm either away or toward the participant. We found that participants demonstrated larger correction magnitudes to visual than tactile target perturbations. Moreover, we found no differences in correction latency between movements to perturbed tactile and visual targets. Further, we found that while participants detected tactile stimuli earlier than visual stimuli, they took longer to initiate reaching movements to an unperturbed tactile target than an unperturbed visual target. These results provide evidence that additional processes may be required when planning movements to tactile versus visual targets and that corrections to changes in tactile target positions alone may not facilitate the latency and magnitude advantages observed for corrections to somatosensory targets (i.e., proprioceptive-tactile targets).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The datasets collected and used for the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  • Bernier PM, Gauthier GM, Blouin J (2007) Evidence for distinct, differentially adaptable sensorimotor transformations for reaches to visual and proprioceptive targets. J Neurophysiol 98(3):1815–1819

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandes J, Heed T (2015) Reach trajectories characterize tactile localization for sensorimotor decision making. J Neurosci 35(40):13648–13658

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bremmer F, Schlack A, Duhamel JR, Graf W, Fink GR (2001) Space coding in primate posterior parietal cortex. Neuroimage 14(1):S46–S51

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brenner E, Smeets JB (1997) Fast responses of the human hand to changes in target position. J Mot Behav 29(4):297–310

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cholewiak RW (1999) The perception of tactile distance: Influences of body site, space, and time. Perception 28(7):851–875

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cressman EK, Franks IM, Enns JT, Chua R (2006) No automatic pilot for visually guided aiming based on colour. Exp Brain Res 171(2):174–183

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donders FC (1868) Die schnelligkeit psychischer processe: Erster artikel. Archiv für Anatomie, Physiologie und wissenschaftliche Medicin, pp 657–681

  • Fiori F, Longo MR (2018) Tactile distance illusions reflect a coherent stretch of tactile space. Proc Natl Acad Sci 115(6):1238–1243

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin DW, Reichenbach A, Franklin S, Diedrichsen J (2016) Temporal evolution of spatial computations for visuomotor control. J Neurosci 36(8):2329–2341

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Green BG (1982) The perception of distance and location for dual tactile pressures. Percept Psychophys 31(4):315–323

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Longo MR (2017) Hand posture modulates perceived tactile distance. Sci Rep 7(1):1–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manson GA, Blouin J, Kumawat AS, Crainic VA, Tremblay L (2019) Rapid online corrections for upper limb reaches to perturbed somatosensory targets: evidence for non-visual sensorimotor transformation processes. Exp Brain Res 237(3):839–853

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marks LE (1983) Similarities and differences among the senses. Int J Neurosci 19(1–4):1–11

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller S, Fiehler K (2016) Mixed body-and gaze-centered coding of proprioceptive reach targets after effector movement. Neuropsychologia 87:63–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ng AW, Chan AH (2012) Finger response times to visual, auditory and tactile modality stimuli. In: Proceedings of the international multiconference of engineers and computer scientists, vol 2, pp 1449–1454

  • Nguyen EH, Taylor JL, Brooks J, Seizova-Cajic T (2016) Velocity of motion across the skin influences perception of tactile location. J Neurophysiol 115(2):674–684

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9(1):97–113

  • Pack CC, Bensmaia SJ (2015) Seeing and feeling motion: canonical computations in vision and touch. PLoS Biol 13(9):e1002271

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pritchett LM, Carnevale MJ, Harris LR (2012) Reference frames for coding touch location depend on the task. Exp Brain Res 222(4):437–445

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pruszynski JA, Johansson RS, Flanagan JR (2016) A rapid tactile-motor reflex automatically guides reaching toward handheld objects. Curr Biol 26(6):788–792

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reichenbach A, Thielscher A, Peer A, Bülthoff HH, Bresciani JP (2009) Seeing the hand while reaching speeds up on-line responses to a sudden change in target position. J Physiol 587(19):4605–4616

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sarlegna FR, Sainburg RL (2007) The effect of target modality on visual and proprioceptive contributions to the control of movement distance. Exp Brain Res 176(2):267–280

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sarlegna FR, Przybyla A, Sainburg RL (2009) The influence of target sensory modality on motor planning may reflect errors in sensori-motor transformations. Neuroscience 164(2):597–610

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders JA, Knill DC (2003) Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 152(3):341–352

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott SH, Cluff T, Lowrey CR, Takei T (2015) Feedback control during voluntary motor actions. Curr Opin Neurobiol 33:85–94

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shore DI, Spry E, Spence C (2002) Confusing the mind by crossing the hands. Cogn Brain Res 14(1):153–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smeets JB, Brenner E (1995) Perception and action are based on the same visual information: distinction between position and velocity. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21(1):19

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sober SJ, Sabes PN (2005) Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning. Nat Neurosci 8(4):490–497

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Van De Lagemaat JM, Kuling IA, Visell Y (2018) Tactile distances are greatly underestimated in perception and motor reproduction. In: 2018 IEEE haptics symposium (HAPTICS), pp 301–306. IEEE

  • Weber EH, Ross HE (1978) The sense of touch. Academic Press for [the] Experimental Psychology Society

  • Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2001) Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing. Nat Neurosci 4(7):759–765

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

Ontario Ministry of Research and Innovation (Grant no: 302419); Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (Grant no: RGPIN-2020-05831); Canada Foundation for Innovation (Grant no: 302419).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Luc Tremblay.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest with regard to the present research.

Additional information

Communicated by Melvyn A. Goodale.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Abdulrabba, S., Tremblay, L. & Manson, G.A. Investigating the online control of goal-directed actions to a tactile target on the body. Exp Brain Res 240, 2773–2782 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-022-06445-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-022-06445-0

Keywords

Navigation