Skip to main content
Log in

Decreasing perceived optic flow rigidity increases postural sway

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Optic flow simulating self-motion through the environment can induce postural adjustments in observers. Some studies investigating this phenomenon have used optic flow patterns increasing in speed from center to periphery, whereas others used optic flow patterns with a constant speed. However, altering the speed gradient of an optic flow stimulus changes the perceived rigidity of such a stimulus. Optic flow stimuli that are perceived as rigid can be expected to provide a stronger sensation of self-motion than non-rigid optic flow, and this may well be reflected in the amount of postural sway. The current study, therefore, examined, by manipulating the speed gradient, to what extent the rigidity of an optic flow stimulus influences posture along the anterior–posterior axis. We used radial random dot expanding or contracting optic flow patterns with three different speed profiles (single-speed, linear speed gradient or quadratic speed gradient) that differentially induce the sensation of self-motion. Interestingly, most postural sway was observed for the non-rigid single-speed optic flow pattern, which contained the least self-motion information of the three profiles. Moreover, we found an anisotropy in that contracting optic flow produced more postural sway than expanding optic flow. In addition, the amount of postural sway increased with increasing stimulus speed, but for contracting optic flow only. Taken together, the results of the current study support the view that visual and sensorimotor systems appear to be tailored toward compensating for rigid optic flow stimulation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A separate set of 8 observers were used for stimulus validation (see Table 1).

  2. For stimulus validation, only two speeds (6 and 24 deg/s) were used.

References

  • Andersen GJ, Dyre BP (1989) Spatial orientation from optic flow in the central visual field. Percept Psychophys 45(4):453–458

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bronstein AM (1986) Suppression of visually evoked postural responses. Exp Brain Res 63(3):655–658

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bubka A, Bonato F, Palmisano S (2008) Expanding and contracting optic-flow patterns and vection. Perception 37(5):704–711

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Bruyn B, Orban GA (1990) The importance of velocity gradients in the perception of three-dimensional rigidity. Perception 19(1):21–27

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards M, Badcock DR (1993) Asymmetries in the sensitivity to motion in depth: a centripetal bias. Perception 22(9):1013–1023

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards M, Ibbotson MR (2007) Relative sensitivities to large-field optic-flow patterns varying in direction and speed. Perception 36(1):113–124

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards M, O’Mahony S, Ibbotson MR, Kohlhagen S (2010) Vestibular stimulation affects optic-flow sensitivity. Perception 39(10):1303–1310

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eklund G, Lofstedt L (1970) Biomechanical analysis of balance. Biomed Eng (NY) 5:333–337

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Gielen CCAM, van Asten WNJC (1990) Postural responses to simulated moving environments are not invariant for the direction of gaze. Exp Brain Res 79:167–174

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Held R (1961) Exposure-history as a factor in maintaining stability of perception and coordination. J Nerv Ment Dis 132:26–32

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Koenderink JJ (1986) Optic flow. Vision Res 26(1):161–180

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN (1980) The optic flow field: the foundation of vision. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 290:169–179

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN, Aronson E (1974) Visual proprioceptive control of standing in human infants. Percept Psychophys 15(3):529–532

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lestienne F, Soechting J, Berthoz A (1977) Postural readjustments induced by linear motion of visual scenes. Exp Brain Res 28(3–4):363–384

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lishman JR, Lee DN (1973) The autonomy of visual kinaesthesis. Perception 2(3):287–294

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Masson G, Mestre DR, Pailhous J (1995) Effects of the spatio-temporal structure of optical flow on postural readjustments in man. Exp Brain Res 103(1):137–150

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura S (2010) Additional oscillation can facilitate visually induced self-motion perception: the effects of its coherence and amplitude gradient. Perception 39:320–329

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palmisano S, Pinniger GJ, Ash A, Steele JR (2009) Effects of simulated viewpoint jitter on visually induced postural sway. Perception 38(3):442–453

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palmisano S, Kim J, Freeman TC (2012) Horizontal fixation point oscillation and simulated viewpoint oscillation both increase vection in depth. J Vis 12(12):15. doi:10.1167/12.12.15

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterka RJ, Benolken MS (1995) Role of somatosensory and vestibular cues in attenuating visually induced human postural sway. Exp Brain Res 105(1):101–110

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schiff W, Caviness JA, Gibson JJ (1962) Persistent fear responses in rhesus monkeys to the optical stimulus of “looming”. Science 136(3520):982–983

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shirai N, Birtles D, Wattam-Bell J, Yamaguchi MK, Kanazawa S, Atkinson J, Braddick O (2009) Asymmetrical cortical processing of radial expansion/contraction in infants and adults. Dev Sci 12(6):946–955. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00839.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen TA (1986) The role of optical velocity in the control of stance. Percept Psychophys 39(5):355–360

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Todd J (1982) Visual information about rigid and nonrigid motion: a geometric analysis. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 8(2):238–252

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Warren WH Jr, Hannon DJ (1988) Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow. Nature 336(6195):162–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wei K, Stevenson IH, Kording KP (2010) The uncertainty associated with visual flow fields and their influence on postural sway: Weber’s law suffices to explain the nonlinearity of vection. J Vis 10(14):4. doi:10.1167/10.14.4

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Vivian Holten.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Example movie of an expanding single-speed optic flow pattern containing a constant angular speed, resulting in the visual percept of which the center appears to move faster than the periphery. Note that the speed of the optic flow stimulus is different than in the experiment, since in the experiment the stimulus subtended 87° by 56°. (MPG 7762 kb)

Example movie of an expanding optic flow pattern containing a linear speed gradient, simulating the motion of a fronto-parallel plane toward the observer. Note that the speed of the optic flow stimulus is different than in the experiment, since in the experiment the stimulus subtended 87° by 56° (MPG 7442 kb)

Example movie of an expanding optic flow pattern containing a quadratic speed gradient, simulating observer movement through a circular tunnel. Note that the speed of the optic flow stimulus is different than in the experiment, since in the experiment the stimulus subtended 87° by 56° (MPG 6428 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Holten, V., Donker, S.F., Verstraten, F.A.J. et al. Decreasing perceived optic flow rigidity increases postural sway. Exp Brain Res 228, 117–129 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3543-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3543-z

Keywords

Navigation