Introduction

Observations on neurological patients exhibiting the spatial neglect syndrome after brain damage, as studied for instance by Pizzamiglio and his colleagues (e.g., Bisiach et al. 1996; Guariglia et al. 1993; Robertson et al. 1997), have provided crucial lines of evidence on the brain basis of spatial cognition and attention. Understanding and treating the disabling deficits of such patients (e.g., Pizzamiglio et al. 1990, 1992, 1998) is of major importance in its own right, as considered by some of the other contributions in this special issue (e.g., Cappa and Perani, this volume). Studies of neglect patients have also shed light on the network of brain areas that may be implicated in normal spatial cognition and attention, based on the typical (and often extensive) lesions in neglect. Lesion evidence implicates an extensive network of brain areas in neglect. The regions typically affected include frontal, parietal and superior temporal cortex, primarily in the right-hemisphere, plus underlying and interconnecting white-matter (e.g., Karnath et al. 2001, 2004; Mort et al. 2003; Vallar 2001; Verdon et al. 2009). See Fig. 1 for a schematic illustration.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic lateral view of the right-hemisphere of the human brain (adapted from Vuilleumier and Driver 2007, with authors’ permission), highlighting some of the many cortical brain regions that have been implicated for possible roles in the unilateral neglect syndrome, based on patients’ lesions. Similar regions have also been implicated in attention-related networks, as based on functional neuroimaging in neurologically intact subjects. Indeed, as noted in our main text, the extensive networks implicated by both types of evidence often show considerable overlap. Important subcortical structures and white-matter connections are omitted for simplicity. The four arrows at top point schematically to some of the sites targeted in the recent concurrent TMS-fMRI studies that we review in the main text. Here, a indicates the right frontal eye fields TMS site of Ruff et al. (2006); b indicates the right intra-parietal TMS site of Ruff et al. (2008); c indicates the right posterior parietal TMS site of Blankenburg et al. (2010) and d indicates the more anterior parietal site of Blankenburg et al. (2008) that was selected to closely match Seyal et al. (1995). We note in passing that these sites all represent fairly ‘dorsal’ sites within the extensive network that we schematically depict here. Future studies by our own group and others will extend the concurrent TMS-fMRI approach to other potential TMS sites, including more inferior regions

A second line of evidence, in which Pizzamiglio and colleagues have also long been active (e.g., see Vallar et al. 1999; Galati et al. 2000), concerns functional neuroimaging data, typically PET or fMRI data, from neurologically healthy subjects as they carry out specific cognitive tasks. This very different type of evidence has also often suggested that extensive brain networks may underlie spatial cognition (e.g., Vallar et al. 1999) and spatial attention (e.g., Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Driver et al. 2004). Moreover, there has often been considerable overlap between those brain networks found in fMRI studies of spatial cognition and attention in healthy participants, with those implicated by the extensive brain lesions of typical neglect patients (e.g., Husain and Rorden 2003; Driver et al. 2004); see again the schematic in Fig. 1. Taken together, such lines of evidence from lesioned patients or functional neuroimaging in neurologically intact subjects have led to the emerging view that rather than single brain areas being identified with single cognitive functions, cognition may be subserved by extended networks of interconnected brain areas (e.g., see Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Driver et al. 2009; Ruff et al. 2009a).

On the other hand, studies that target specific brain areas more selectively have led to suggestions that a given area may play an essential role in a specific cognitive process. In research with human participants, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) has often been used to target specific brain areas (provided they are near enough to the surface to be approached by TMS) in order to test such hypotheses (e.g., for reviews, see Walsh and Pascual-Leone 2005; Wassermann et al. 2008). In rare cases where invasive electrodes were available for surgical reasons, targeted invasive stimulations have also been possible in a few human patients (e.g., Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2005), somewhat analogous to microstimulation work in non-human primates (e.g., see Cohen and Newsome 2004). Such interventional approaches have also been supplemented with conventional focal lesion work, either by selecting human patients with unusually focal damage (e.g., Husain et al. 2003), or by experimental (often reversible) lesions in animals (e.g., Lomber and Galuske 2002). These more targeted approaches have led to hypotheses that specific single brain areas may make unique contributions to particular cognitive processes, such as for those aspects of spatial cognition and attention that we consider here. But given the coexisting evidence for more extensive networks of interconnected brain areas (see above and Fig. 1), this then raises the question of how an implicated specific brain area may interact with closely interconnected regions within the same network, as a function of the current task requirements.

In this context, several sophisticated analysis approaches have been developed for application to standard fMRI, EEG or MEG data, which seek to uncover patterns of functional influences or ‘effective connectivity’ between interconnected brain regions. Such patterns of effective connectivity may relate to particular cognitive processes and may even change dynamically in a task-dependent manner (e.g., Friston et al. 2003; Goebel et al. 2003; Schnitzler and Gross 2005; Valdés-Sosa et al. 2005). But here we focus instead on more interventional approaches that target a particular brain area with a causal intervention (e.g., TMS, or microstimulation or even a permanent lesion), while studying the impact on brain function in remote but interconnected areas within a more extended brain network.

Combining TMS with concurrent fMRI to study causal influences in the brain networks subserving attention and spatial cognition

TMS has been combined with PET (e.g., Fox et al. 1997; Paus et al. 1997; Siebner et al. 1999), but this does not allow the same temporal or anatomical resolution as fMRI. Pioneering studies established the feasibility of combining TMS with fMRI about 10 years ago (e.g., see Bohning et al. 1999). The technical challenges for successfully combining TMS with concurrent fMRI are considerable and are reviewed elsewhere (e.g., Bestmann et al. 2008; Bohning et al. 2003; Siebner et al. 2009; Weiskopf et al. 2009). While the BOLD signals revealed by fMRI may not index all forms of neural activity (Logothetis 2008) and may be insensitive to lower intensity TMS (Bohning et al. 1999), concurrent TMS-fMRI offers the advantage of potentially tracking the causal impact on many brain areas of TMS applied to one or other targeted site. Space constraints preclude an exhaustive review of all concurrent TMS-fMRI studies here. Instead we focus on use of concurrent TMS-fMRI to study the brain basis of spatial attention and spatial cognition, initially describing our own work and then expanding the focus to include potentially related studies.

The frontal eye fields (FEF) have long been implicated as an important node in the so-called ‘dorsal attention network’ (e.g., Corbetta and Shulman 2002), based on converging evidence from patients (e.g., Henik et al. 1994), and from experimental lesions and inactivation in non-human primates (e.g., Latto and Cowey 1971; Wardak et al. 2006); plus single-cell recordings (e.g., Juan et al. 2004) and purely behavioural TMS work in humans (Grosbras and Paus 2002; Müri and Nyffeler 2008; Neggers et al. 2007; Silvanto et al. 2006). Such work had led to many suggestions that the FEF might play a causal role in modulating selective visual processing. Ruff et al. (2006) set out to test this by combining TMS over right human FEF with concurrent fMRI of retinotopic visual cortex. They found (see Fig. 2a, b) that as the intensity of TMS over right FEF was increased, this affected BOLD signal in occipital visual cortex, for retinotopic areas V1–V4. Specifically, this increased BOLD signal there for representations of the peripheral retinotopic visual field, while having the opposite effect (of decreasing BOLD instead) for retinotopic representations of the central visual field (see Fig. 2b). This might accord with the FEF over-representing the peripheral visual field (due to the role of the FEF in controlling eye-movements and directing covert attention into the visual periphery), leading to an excitatory impact for the peripheral visual field in interconnected early visual cortex when the FEF are stimulated, but a converse effect for central vision. Ruff et al. (2006) went on to show a psychophysical analogue of the remote BOLD effects on visual cortex due to right FEF TMS. Specifically, such TMS increased the perceived contrast of peripheral visual stimuli, relative to central visual stimuli, paralleling the BOLD findings for human visual cortex. Conceptually similar effects have been observed in the non-human primate brain, as related pioneering work has shown that microstimulation of macaque FEF can lead to corresponding increases in visual responses for single-cells recorded concurrently in V4 (Armstrong and Moore 2007); while more recently FEF microstimulation has been combined with concurrent fMRI in awake monkeys, to reveal modulation of responses in visual cortex (Ekstrom et al. 2008), as well as interconnected parietal cortex.

Fig. 2
figure 2

a and b TMS over human right FEF induces a characteristic pattern of BOLD changes in visual areas V1–V4, with BOLD increases for representations of the visual periphery and BOLD decreases for the central visual field. a shows a 3D rendering of one illustrative participant’s brain, with a schematic TMS coil indicating the approximate TMS site over right FEF (see also Fig. 1a). The cutout slices in occipital cortex show overlays of statistical parametric maps (SPMs) for BOLD changes related to FEF-TMS intensity. Significant activity increases due to increased TMS intensity are marked in red/yellow, significant activity decreases in blue; these two opposite effects correspond to representations of the peripheral versus central visual field, respectively. b shows estimates of right-FEF-TMS effects (T-values coding correlation of BOLD and TMS intensity) on representations of more central or more peripheral representations of the visual field (‘eccentricity sectors’) for visual areas V1–V4, as defined by separate individual retinotopic mapping. Activity increases for the more peripheral visual field, but activity decreases for the central visual field, are found for all retinotopic areas. Note that right FEF TMS had this impact on both right and left visual cortex. c 3D rendering of one illustrative participant’s brain, with a schematic TMS coil indicating the approximate TMS site over right intra-parietal cortex in Ruff et al. (2008). The cutout slices in occipital cortex show overlays of statistical parametric maps (SPMs) for BOLD changes related to FEF-TMS intensity. Significant activity increases due to increased TMS intensity are marked in red. d Estimates of right-IPS-TMS effects (coded as for b) on representations of the more central or more peripheral visual field for visual areas V1–V4, showing increased BOLD signal. Note the different pattern of results to that found with right FEF TMS (compare with b); there is no longer a reduced BOLD signal for representations of the central visual field after right-intraparietal TMS. e Left intra-parietal TMS had no impact on BOLD signals in retinotopic visual cortex. Data for Fig. 2 taken from Ruff et al. (2006, 2008, 2009b), with authors’ permission

In follow-up studies, Ruff and colleagues went on to show that stimulating human intraparietal cortex, rather than FEF, led to a distinct pattern of influence upon visual cortex (Ruff et al. 2008); see Fig. 2c, d. Moreover, they subsequently found some right-hemisphere predominance for these remote functional effects in humans (Ruff et al. 2009b); compare Fig. 2d, e. Notably, left-intraparietal TMS had no impact on BOLD signals in visual cortex, quite unlike the robust effects due to right-intraparietal TMS. Right FEF TMS also had more substantial effects on visual cortex than left-FEF TMS. Such lateralisation appears broadly consistent with that found for purely behavioural TMS effects in several visual tasks with humans (e.g., see Grosbras and Paus 2002; O’Shea et al. 2004; Silvanto et al. 2006). It also accords with the extensive clinical evidence from neglect patients for some right-hemisphere predominance in the networks subserving spatial cognition and attention (e.g., see Karnath et al. 2001, 2004; Mort et al. 2003; Vallar 2001; Verdon et al. 2009). Thus, Ruff et al.’s findings with concurrent TMS-fMRI are consistent with the emerging view that parietal cortex has undergone particular hemispheric specialisation in humans (e.g., see Milner and Goodale 1996).

The concurrent TMS-fMRI studies by Ruff and colleagues demonstrate that human FEF, and human parietal cortex, are capable of modulating the response of early visual cortex, in accord with a potential role for these nodes within the proposed ‘attention network’ (see Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Driver et al. 2004) in gating the response of visual cortex. However, in these initial studies, the top–down attentional state of participants was not manipulated during the concurrent TMS-fMRI. Instead, participants in Ruff et al. (2006, 2008, 2009b) merely had to hold central fixation. More recently, Blankenburg et al. (2010) applied TMS over right posterior parietal cortex (PPC) while varying the attentional state of their participants. These participants always viewed equivalent bilateral stimuli, but either maintained covert spatial attention to the left visual field in order to judge stimuli there; or attended covertly to the right visual field in order to judge stimuli there instead; or attended neutrally by simply pressing a button whenever the stimuli appeared. As shown in Fig. 3, the impact of the right PPC TMS on remote brain activations depended strongly on the current attentional state. It increased the BOLD response of right ventral visual cortex (probable V4) when attending left (see Fig. 3a) yet decreased response in the same region when attending right; while having the opposite impact on (opposite-hemisphere) left ventral visual cortex (see Fig. 3b). By contrast, there was no such remote impact of the right PPC TMS during neutral attention. These results show that TMS-fMRI can reveal remote influences between brain areas that change dynamically with purely top–down attentional factors. We attribute the observed pattern of remote TMS effects to the ‘effective connectivity’ between right PPC and visual cortex changing as a function of the current attentional state, such that stimulation applied over right PPC propagate differentially to interconnected visual cortex, in a manner that changes for different attentional conditions. This evidently can even include inter-hemispheric influences of the right PPC TMS, as for left visual cortex in Fig. 3b.

Fig. 3
figure 3

In the concurrent TMS-fMRI study by Blankenburg et al. (2010), TMS was applied over right posterior parietal cortex PPC (as indicated schematically by lightning-flash symbols in the coronal views here), while subjects directed covert attention to the left hemifield, or to the right hemifield instead, or attended neutrally (see main text). fMRI data were acquired concurrently, with a posterior MR coil to maximise sensitive for visual cortex (hence we did not acquire BOLD data for more anterior brain regions, e.g., in frontal cortex; the imaged posterior volume is indicated with higher contrast here). High- minus low-intensity right PPC TMS increased BOLD signals in a manner that significantly depended on the currently attended side, as shown in the interaction SPMs superimposed on the image volumes here. When attending left versus right, high minus low-intensity right PPC TMS increased BOLD in right extrastriate visual cortex (a), as shown in the coronal and transverse images on the left of the figure here. When attending right versus left, high- versus low-intensity right PPC TMS now increased BOLD in left extrastriate visual cortex instead (b), as shown in the images on the right of the figure here. Thus, which visual hemisphere showed increased BOLD signal for higher intensity TMS reversed, depending on the currently attended side. Adapted with authors’ permission from Blankenburg et al. (2010)

A further concurrent TMS-fMRI study by Blankenburg et al. (2008) also examined the possible inter-hemispheric impact of right parietal TMS, but now for somatosensory rather than visual responses. Seyal et al. (1995) had conducted an earlier pioneering behavioural TMS study in relation to possible ‘hemispheric rivalry’. Such rivalry is a traditional proposal from behavioural neurology, in relation to phenomena such as the neglect syndrome and unilateral extinction (e.g., see Kinsbourne 1977). Seyal et al. reported that right parietal TMS in neurologically healthy subjects could lead to behavioural enhancement of somatosensory detection on the ipsilateral right hand. They tentatively attributed that result to a remote effect from right parietal TMS upon the (opposite) left-hemisphere response to incoming somatosensory stimulation from the right hand. But since they had used no neural measures, they were unable to demonstrate directly the proposed impact on neural responses in the opposite hemisphere. Blankenburg et al. (2008) used concurrent TMS-fMRI to show that right parietal TMS does indeed enhance the response of left somatosensory cortex (including left S1) to the presence versus absence of right-hand (electrical) somatosensory input; see Fig. 4. This study further illustrates that concurrent TMS-fMRI can be used to study inter-hemispheric influences between interconnected brain regions, addressing issues for the normal brain that can also relate to clinical issues concerning neglect and extinction arising from the study of brain-damaged patients, a topic we return to at the end of this paper.

Fig. 4
figure 4

In the concurrent TMS-fMRI study by Blankenburg et al. (2008), TMS was applied over right anterior parietal cortex (as indicated schematically by lightning-flash symbols), in the presence or absence of electrical somatosensory stimulation for the right hand, during concurrent whole-brain fMRI. The fMRI data revealed an inter-hemispheric effect of the right parietal TMS. Specifically, the BOLD signal for left S1 (as indicated on the transverse and coronal views here, for slices taken through the MNI coordinate shown) in response to right-hand somatosensory input was enhanced when combined with high intensity (versus low) TMS. See plot on right for the differential TMS effect (high minus low) as a function of the presence or absence of concurrent right-hand somatosensory input. See main text for discussion of how this relates to the previous purely behavioural TMS findings of Seyal et al. (1995) concerning inter-hemispheric effects of right parietal cortex on left-hemisphere somatosensory processing that might potentially relate to clinical somatosensory extinction or neglect after pathological right-parietal disruption

Several other groups have also combined TMS with fMRI in potentially related work (see e.g., Baudewig et al. 2001; Bohning et al. 2003; Denslow et al. 2005; Kemna and Gembris 2003; Sack et al. 2007 for other concurrent TMS-fMRI examples; and Hubl et al. 2008; O’Shea et al. 2007 for examples of fMRI utilised before and after an intervening off-line, repetitive TMS intervention that was intended to produce a relatively long-lasting disruption). In one illustrative example of concurrent TMS-fMRI, Sack et al. (2007) applied TMS over left or right parietal cortex, during a spatial cognition task (angle judgements, somewhat reminiscent of some of the ‘clock’ tasks often used with neglect patients) or during a non-spatial control task. Right but not left parietal TMS disrupted spatial performance. Concurrent fMRI revealed effects of right but not left parietal TMS for BOLD signals in right parietal and interconnected right frontal cortex that correlated with the behavioural effects.

Combining TMS with concurrent EEG to study the possible causal impact of a targeted brain region upon others

TMS can also be combined with concurrent EEG, which can provide a much finer temporal resolution than fMRI, at the expense of spatial anatomical resolution. Again there are technical issues to overcome (such as the instantaneous electrical artefact during each TMS pulse, plus the ERPs triggered by the associated click-sound and scalp-sensation, etc). These technical challenges are all surmountable (e.g., see Thut and Pascual-Leone 2010; Ilmoniemi and Kicic 2010). Several recent studies have used concurrent TMS-EEG to study possible attention-related effects of TMS over (or near) human FEF, in the context of visual attention paradigms. Taylor et al. (2007) reported that posterior negativities, within ~200 ms of visual target onset, could be modulated by right FEF TMS, in a Posner-like (Posner et al. 1980) spatial precuing paradigm. This was taken to indicate a remote attention-dependent influence on sources in visual cortex. Morishima et al. (2009) reported that TMS over electrode position FC2 (argued by those authors to fall close to human FEF) altered ERPs at occipital electrodes, when participants were forewarned by an early precue that they would have to attend to a face stimulus or a motion stimulus in a composite visual display. These findings were again interpreted as indicating remote attention-dependent influences on sources in visual cortex. Most recently, Capotosto et al. (2009) applied TMS to right frontal eye fields or right intra-parietal sulcus during presentation of a precue that directed attention to a peripheral spatial location, where a target could be presented some seconds later. Concurrent EEG measurements showed that TMS at either cortical site affected anticipatory alpha desynchronisation as measured at parieto–occipital electrodes. Moreover, these effects correlated with response-time slowing for the subsequently presented target. Based on these findings, Capotosto et al. (2009) suggest that FEF and IPS may exert top–down influences on visual processing via neural (de)synchronisation of brain oscillations.

The functional significance of such fairly rapid oscillatory brain phenomena (e.g., from the delta and alpha bands through to beta, gamma and above) can be directly studied with combinations of TMS and EEG, due to the excellent temporal resolution of both techniques. Thut and Miniussi (2009) recently reviewed the possibility of interfering with, or driving, specific brain oscillatory phenomena by using rhythmic TMS at specific frequencies for targeted sites. This remains an exciting direction for future research. Although fMRI data are acquired at a much slower timescale, BOLD signals can also show some (correspondingly slower) oscillatory phenomena (e.g., Fox and Raichle 2007) that might potentially to relate to states of communication among networks of interconnected brain regions (e.g., see Mantini et al. 2007). In the longer-term, TMS might thus be combined not only with EEG but also with fMRI to study the possible causal role of oscillatory neural phenomena (at faster or slower timescales, for EEG or fMRI, respectively) in supporting specific processes such as spatial attention and spatial cognition.

Applying fMRI and/or EEG in focally lesioned patients, to study the possible causal impact of the lesioned brain region upon others

fMRI and EEG can also be applied to brain-damaged patients exhibiting particular neuropsychological symptoms, as in cases of spatial neglect or unilateral extinction. The intention in doing so is not to seek a response from the dead or absent tissue. Rather the aim is to study the possible impact of the lesion upon function in remote surviving regions that might normally interact with the damaged area(s), but then function abnormally when the lesion removes some of the usual influences upon surviving regions. Thus, although chronic brain lesions differ in many respects from application of TMS in healthy participants (as was reviewed above), there is the abstract similarity of being able to look for the remote functional consequences of local brain disruption. Pizzamiglio et al. were among the first to apply functional neuroimaging to neglect patients (e.g., Pizzamiglio et al. 1998). In London, we have used fMRI to study the response of visual cortex to contralateral visual stimuli, in neglect and/or unilateral extinction patients, during unilateral or bilateral visual stimuli. Rees et al. (2000, 2002) reported residual unconscious activation in early right visual cortex for extinguished and/or neglected visual stimuli in the left visual field, together with enhanced responses in surviving parietal and frontal cortex for the same stimuli when consciously detected, in a single case (see also Vuilleumier et al. 2001, 2002; plus Vuilleumier and Driver 2007, for review). Sarri et al. (in press) recently replicated and extended these results to a series of multiple cases of neglect/extinction after right-parietal damage. Marzi et al. (e.g., Marzi et al. 2000, 2001) have made related observations, by using EEG to study ERPs in response to visual stimuli in neglect and/or extinction patients (see also Spinelli et al. 1994). Knight and colleagues have further shown that frontal lesions impact upon visual responses to task-related stimuli, as assessed with ERPs (e.g., Barceló et al. 2000; Yago et al. 2004).

In a recent fMRI study, Vuilleumier et al. (2008) studied two patients with enduring left neglect and extinction after relatively focal right-parietal injury. They varied the ‘attentional load’ of a task at fixation, while studying the possible impact of this on the BOLD response of retinotopic visual cortex to task-irrelevant but salient peripheral checkerboards in either visual hemifield (see Fig. 5a). In healthy participants, increased attentional load at fixation slightly reduces the BOLD response of visual cortex to the peripheral checkerboards (Schwartz et al. 2005) but does so symmetrically for the response of either visual hemisphere to a contralateral checkerboard. By contrast, in the two right-parietal neglect patients, the visual hemispheres responded normally and symmetrically under low attentional load at fixation (see Fig. 5b), but a pathological asymmetry emerged when attentional load at fixation was increased. For retinotopically mapped visual areas (see Fig. 5c), the BOLD response of (intact) right visual cortex in the damaged hemisphere to left hemifield checkerboards was now pathologically reduced (even eliminated for V4, see Fig. 5d upper two plots); while there was (abnormally) no impact of attentional load at fixation on the BOLD response of left visual cortex to a right hemifield checkerboard (see Fig. 5d, lower two plots). As discussed by Vuilleumier et al. (2008), the emergence of a pathological asymmetry in visual cortex function after right-parietal injury, specifically when attentional demand at current fixation is increased, can explain several otherwise paradoxical aspects of the unilateral visual neglect syndrome, including its apparently labile nature. Vuilleumier et al. (2008) went on to identify a behavioural analogue of their BOLD results. But for present purposes, the key point of this study lies in demonstrating a remote functional abnormality in intact visual cortex, after right-parietal disruption, that depended specifically on the current attentional state. In this sense, their patients’ results have some abstract parallels with the results of Blankenburg et al. (2010), who demonstrated with TMS-fMRI in healthy people that remote influences of right parietal cortex on visual cortex can depend on the current attentional state (cf. Fig. 3 here).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Schematic paradigm and neuroimaging results from the patient fMRI study of Vuilleumier et al. (2008) that found an attention-dependent impact of right-parietal lesions on function in remote visual cortex, for two neglect patients. Paradigm shown in (a): The patients performed a low- or high-load task at central fixation, for a successive stream of coloured characters there. Task-irrelevant checkerboards could appear in left or right visual field (LVF/RVF), as illustrated. Under low-load at fixation, patients show preserved response of visual cortex to contralateral checkerboards (see whole-brain SPMs in panel B for two different patients, with the upper row showing preserved response of right visual cortex to LVF, and the lower row showing the symmetric response of left visual cortex to RVF). Note also the right-parietal lesion of each patient (arrowed). Retinotopic regions V1–V4 were functionally mapped separately in each patient (see panel C). Despite the evident preservation of functional retinotopic visual cortex in both visual hemispheres for both patients, the right-parietal lesions imposed an attention-dependent abnormality upon the responses of visual cortex, as follows. While visual cortex functioned normally under low-load at fixation in the patients, when attentional load at fixation was increased, a pathological asymmetry emerged (see histograms in panel D). Increased attentional load at fixation drastically reduced the contralateral response to LVF checkerboards in right V1–V4 even eliminating this response for right V4, as if that now became blind (see upper row of histograms in panel (D), one for each illustrated patient). But increased attentional load at fixation had no impact on left V1–V4 [see lower row of histograms in (D)]. These results reveal an attention-dependent abnormal asymmetry in visual cortex function, after right-parietal damage. See main text for discussion. Adapted with authors’ permission from Vuilleumier et al. (2008)

Other groups have also applied fMRI to neglect and/or extinction patients. For instance, Corbetta et al. (2005) and He et al. (2007) reported in a group of neglect patients that task-evoked BOLD responses (Corbetta et al. 2005), or functional connectivity between regions within attention-related extended brain networks as measured with fMRI (He et al. 2007), showed systematic abnormalities within surviving intact regions remote from the lesion. These related to performance in a spatial precuing attention task, and also to the clinical severity/recovery of individual patients. Hyper-activity within left parietal and frontal cortex, contralateral to the damaged hemisphere, contributed to this pattern. Such contralesional hyper-activity in neglect had long been hypothesised on clinical and theoretical grounds (e.g., Kinsbourne 1977) but had rarely if ever been demonstrated directly hitherto (though see Koch et al. 2008).

Pizzamiglio et al. have expanded considerable effort on possible rehabilitation strategies for neglect over many years (e.g., see Pizzamiglio et al. 1990, 1992; Antonucci et al. 1995). One possible strategy follows up on the possibility of hyper-excitability within the undamaged (usually left) hemisphere, as mentioned above. This can be approached using repetitive TMS protocols over the intact hemisphere that aim to reduce such hyper-excitability. The rationale here is that if such hyper-excitability can be returned to normal levels, some of the neglect and/or extinction symptoms might be alleviated. After some initial positive demonstrations for extinction (e.g., Oliveri et al. 1999), there have been further recent developments in the application of TMS as a potential therapy for neglect. These include a recent (Koch et al. 2008) direct demonstration, with twin-coil TMS, of hyper-excitability within parietal-motor networks for the intact left-hemisphere of neglect patients. Moreover, this same study observed that repetitive TMS applied over that intact hemisphere reduced this hyper-excitability back to the normal range and led in parallel to corresponding improvements in neglect symptom (e.g., see Koch et al. 2008).

TMS can now be combined with fMRI and with EEG, as we have reviewed above, to reveal the impact of interventions targeting a specific brain area for function in remote interconnected areas. These combined approaches are now sufficiently well established that they can also begin to be applied to brain-damaged patients. Moreover, TMS already shows exciting possibilities for remediation of neglect and extinction (e.g., Koch et al. 2008; Oliveri et al. 1999); while the available TMS interventions should increasingly be informed by the emerging literature on how rhythmic TMS may affect ongoing brain oscillations (e.g., see Thut and Miniussi 2009). Taking all of these points together, we anticipate that the various different strands of research that we have briefly summarised above are likely to converge in the future, to allow further advances on the topics that Pizzamiglio has studied and highlighted throughout his career. The new methodological combinations allow a distinct approach to enduring questions, while also raising many new questions about how separate but connected brain areas may interact within the normal and damaged human brain.