Skip to main content
Log in

Vision of the body increases interference on the somatic signal detection task

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research suggests that attention has a significant effect on somatic perception in both healthy people and those who suffer from somatic disturbance. The current study investigates the effects of attending to the body on somatic awareness and a particular type of somatic disturbance: erroneous reports of touch sensation, as measured by the Somatic Signal Detection Task (SSDT). During the SSDT, participants are required to detect near-threshold tactile stimulation at their fingertip. Previous research has found that healthy participants erroneously report touch sensations in the absence of a stimulus on this task and that such false alarms are increased when a simultaneous light flash is presented next to their fingertip. Thirty-seven participants completed the SSDT under two conditions: non-informative vision of the hand and no vision of the hand. False alarms were significantly higher in light trials in the non-informative vision condition compared to light trials in the no-vision condition. However, hit rates, sensitivity (d′) and response criterion (c) were not affected by non-informative vision of the hand. Using the SSDT, we found that viewing the body increased somatic interference, possibly due to raised awareness of internal bodily sensations. This work provides evidence that viewing the body can have a detrimental effect on simple detection of near-threshold tactile stimulation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Five participants had hit rates of over 90% and one had a hit rate of 1% during no light trials in the non-informative vision condition of the SSDT (equivalent to thresholding trials) suggesting that a consistent threshold level had not been achieved for these participants.

  2. The original intention was to use confidence ratings to conduct receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis, however this was not possible as some participants did not use all of the response options in all conditions. Collapsing the data gave similar false alarm rates as found in previous studies (e.g. Lloyd et al. 2008).

  3. Although false alarm rates were higher in the vision compared to the no vision condition in both light conditions, this increase was only significant in the light condition.

References

  • Ahles TA, Cassens HL, Stalling RB (1987) Private body consciousness, anxiety and the perception of pain. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 18:215–222

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing Inc, Arlington

    Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick M, Cohen J (1998) Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature 391:756

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chambers CD, Payne JM, Mattingley JB (2007) Parietal disruption impairs reflexive spatial attention within and between sensory modalities. Neuropsychologia 45:1715–1724

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cornsweet TN (1962) The staircase method in psychophysics. Am J Psychol 75:485–491

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Deary V, Chalder T, Sharpe M (2007) The cognitive behavioural model of medically unexplained symptoms: a theoretical and empirical review. Clin Psychol Rev 27:781–797

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ghazanfar AA, Schroeder CE (2006) Is neocortex essentially multisensory? Trends Cogn Sci 10:278–285

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano MSA, Gross CG (1993) A bimodal map of space—somatosensory receptive fields in the macaque putamen with corresponding visual receptive fields. Exp Brain Res 97:96–109

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano MSA, Yap GS, Gross CG (1994) Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. Science 266:1054–1057

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Haenen MA, Schmidt AJM, Kroeze S, vandenHout MA (1996) Hypochondriasis and symptom reporting—the effect of attention versus distraction. Psychother Psychosom 65:43–48

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Haggard P, Christakou A, Serino A (2007) Viewing the body modulates tactile receptive fields. Exp Brain Res 180:187–193

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halligan PW, Hunt M, Marshall JC, Wade DT (1996) When seeing is feeling: acquired synaesthesia or phantom touch? Neurocase 2:21–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Hunt M, Wade DT (1997) Somatosensory assessment: can seeing produce feeling? J Neurol 244:199–203

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Harris JA, Arabzadeh E, Moore CA, Clifford CWG (2007) Noninformative vision causes adaptive changes in tactile sensitivity. J Neurosci 27:7136–7140

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson RM, Burton PC, Ro T (2006) Visually induced feelings of touch. Brain Res 1073:398–406

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kennett S, Taylor-Clarke M, Haggard P (2001) Noninformative vision improves the spatial resolution of touch in humans. Curr Biol 11:1188–1191

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kennett S, Spence C, Driver J (2002) Visuo-tactile links in covert exogenous spatial attention remap across changes in unseen hand posture. Percept Psychophys 64:1083–1094

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ladavas E, di Pellegrino G, Farne A, Zeloni G (1998a) Neuropsychological evidence of an integrated visuo-tactile representation of peripersonal space in humans. Eur J Neurosci 10:254

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladavas E, Zeloni G, Farne A (1998b) Visual peripersonal space centred on the face in humans. Brain 121:2317–2326

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ladavas E, Farne A, Zeloni G, di Pellegrino G (2000) Seeing or not seeing where your hands are. Exp Brain Res 131:458–467

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd DM, Shore DI, Spence C, Calvert GA (2003) Multisensory representation of limb position in human premotor cortex. Nat Neurosci 6:17–18

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd DM, Mason L, Brown RJ, Poliakoff E (2008) Development of a paradigm for measuring somatic disturbance in clinical populations with medically unexplained symptoms. J Psychosom Res 64:21–24

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mackay WA, Crammond DJ (1987) Neuronal correlates in posterior parietal lobe of the expectation of events. Behav Brain Res 24:167–179

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Macmillan NA, Creelman CD (1991) Detection theory: a user’s guide. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller LC, Murphy R, Buss AH (1981) Consciousness of body—private and public. J Pers Soc Psychol 41:397–406

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moss-Morris R, Sharon C, Tobin R, Baldi JC (2005) A randomized controlled graded exercise trial for chronic fatigue syndrome: outcomes and mechanisms of change. J Health Psychol 10:245–259

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield RC (1971) Assessment and analysis of handedness—Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Press C, Taylor-Clarke M, Kennett S, Haggard P (2004) Visual enhancement of touch in spatial body representation. Exp Brain Res 154:238–245

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran VS, Hirstein W (1998) The perception of phantom limbs: The D. O. Hebb lecture. Brain J Neurol 121(9):1603–1630

    Google Scholar 

  • Rief W, Barsky AJ (2005) Psychobiological perspectives on somatoform disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30:996–1002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rief W, Broadbent E (2007) Explaining medically unexplained symptoms-models and mechanisms. Clin Psychol Rev 27:821–841

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Scandolara C, Matelli M, Gentilucci M (1981) Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. 2. Visual responses. Behav Brain Res 2:147–163

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt AJM, Wolfstakens DJ, Oosterlaan J, Vandenhout MA (1994) Psychological mechanisms in hypochondriasis—attention-induced physical symptoms without sensory stimulation. Psychother Psychosom 61:117–120

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schroeder CE, Foxe J (2005) Multisensory contributions to low-level, ‘unisensory’ processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:454–458

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Serino A, Haggard P (2010) Touch and the body. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:224–236

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Serino A, Farne A, Rinaldesi ML, Haggard P, Ladavas E (2007) Can vision of the body ameliorate impaired somatosensory function? Neuropsychologia 45:1101–1107

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Serino A, Padiglioni S, Haggard P, Ladavas E (2009) Seeing the hand boosts feeling on the cheek. Cortex 45:602–609

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass JG, Corwin J (1988) Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. J Exp Psychol Gen 117:34–50

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spence C, Nicholls MER, Gillespie N, Driver J (1998a) Cross-modal links in exogenous covert spatial orienting between touch, audition, and vision. Percept Psychophys 60:544–557

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spence C, Pavani F, Driver J (1998b) What crossing the hands can reveal about crossmodal links in spatial attention. Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society. 3

  • Spence C, Pavani F, Driver J (2004) Spatial constraints on visual-tactile cross-modal distracter congruency effects. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4:148–169

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor-Clarke M, Kennett S, Haggard P (2004) Persistence of visual-tactile enhancement in humans. Neurosci Lett 354:22–25

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper SP, Lloyd D, Shorland B, Dancer C, Howard LA, McGlone F (1998) Vision influences tactile perception without proprioceptive orienting. Neuroreport 9:1741–1744

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weisz J, Balazs L, Adam G (1988) The influence of self-focused attention on heartbeat perception. Psychophysiology 25:193–199

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou YD, Fuster JM (2000) Visuo-tactile cross-modal associations in cortical somatosensory cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:9777–9782

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by grants from The Leverhulme Trust [F/00 120/BF] and the BBSRC (BB/D524432/1). The authors wish to thank Dr Kirsten McKenzie for help in programming the experiment.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura Mirams.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mirams, L., Poliakoff, E., Brown, R.J. et al. Vision of the body increases interference on the somatic signal detection task. Exp Brain Res 202, 787–794 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2185-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2185-7

Keywords

Navigation