Introduction

The behavioral pharmacology of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA or ecstasy), a drug that has emerged as a significant drug of abuse over the last decade, has been studied in both nonhuman species and humans (see review by Green et al. 2003). MDMA is a phenylethylamine related structurally to both the amphetamines and mescaline (Green et al. 2003). In an attempt to differentiate the relative roles of the different neurotransmitter systems in the acute effects of MDMA, a variety of studies have been done in animals. In studies using measures of in vitro and in vivo neurochemistry, MDMA has been shown to be a potent releaser of both serotonin (5-HT) and dopamine (DA) (Cole and Sumnall 2003a,b; Green et al. 2003; Koch and Galloway 1997; Schmidt et al. 1987; White et al. 1996). Behavioral studies have been conducted using self-administration and drug discrimination procedures. While there are studies of the mediation of the reinforcing effects of MDMA in nonhuman primates (e.g., Fantegrossi et al. 2002), animal studies in rats, pigeons, and rhesus monkeys have largely focused on the discriminative stimulus effects of MDMA as a model of its subjective effects in humans and often considered a marker for reinforcing effects and hence related to abuse (Baker and Taylor 1997). These studies have determined that both amphetamine and 5-HT-mediated drugs, including fenfluramine and LSD, substitute in animals trained to discriminate MDMA from placebo and vice versa (Baker et al. 1995; Baker and Taylor 1997; Evans and Johanson 1986; Goodwin and Baker 2000; Goodwin et al. 2003; Kamien et al. 1986; Oberlender and Nichols 1988; Schechter 1986). Studies using a three-way discrimination to more finely differentiate MDMA’s effects have demonstrated a pattern of results using substitution tests and pretreatments with DA and 5-HT antagonists suggesting that MDMAs discriminative stimulus effects are more 5-HT-like, although DA systems may also be involved (Goodwin and Baker 2000; Goodwin et al. 2003).

Several studies in humans have shown that the acute administration of MDMA produces a unique profile of subjective effects suggesting an interaction between dopaminergic and serotonergic systems in mediating these effects (Cami et al. 2000; Tancer and Johanson 2001, 2003; Vollenweider et al. 1998). Tancer and Johanson (2001, 2003) compared the subjective effects of a range of doses of MDMA to both d-amphetamine, a prototypic dopaminergic drug, and metachlorophenylpiperazine (mCPP), a 5-HT-releasing agent. These studies demonstrated that the profile of reinforcing and subjective effects of MDMA was similar to both d-amphetamine and mCPP. These investigators also conducted a three-way drug discrimination study similar to those conducted in rats (Johanson et al. 2006). They trained humans to discriminate among d-amphetamine, mCPP, and placebo and found that half of the participants discriminated 1 and 1.5 mg/kg MDMA as d-amphetamine and half as mCPP (Johanson et al. 2006). In addition, the subjective effects reported showed that MDMA did indeed have a mixed profile as shown in previous studies.

Another strategy for determining the role of neurotransmitters is to manipulate the actions of the drug with agonists and antagonist drugs and measure changes in the targeted behavior. As noted above, drug discrimination studies in nonhumans using this strategy have led to the conclusion that MDMA is mediated by both DA and 5-HT. In humans, these studies are relatively difficult to conduct as highly specific agonists and antagonists are often not available for administration to humans. Nevertheless, Liechti et al. (2000a,b) have done several studies implicating a prominent role for 5-HT in the actions of MDMA in humans using acute administration of a 5-HT2 antagonist, ketanserin, and an SSRI, citalopram. However, this group also demonstrated that haloperidol, a DA receptor antagonist, also could modify the subjective effects of MDMA (Liechti and Vollenweider 2000a). In the present study, these results were expanded using fluoxetine, another SSRI, which has been shown to antagonize the effects of MDMA in animal studies (Baumann et al. 2005; Berger et al. 1992; Mechan et al. 2002), most likely due to its blockade of MDMA’s access to the 5-HT transporter, preventing a release of 5-HT.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants who were recruited were between 18 and 35 years of age, had a minimum of a high school education or GED, had no medical condition that precluded participation, and were under 91 kg. In addition, they had to have a history of stimulant recreational drug use (at least six times) including previous MDMA use (at least three times) with at least one use in the last 6 months. No candidate with a current or past Axis I diagnosis including drug or alcohol dependence disorder, other than nicotine dependence, was eligible. The exception was that drug or alcohol abuse disorder was not exclusionary if at least a year had passed since remission. Candidates were excluded if lifetime recreational use of psychomotor stimulants, opiates, phencyclidine, or sedatives exceeded 50 times. Women could not be pregnant or lactating. The study was approved by the Wayne State University Human Investigation Committee, and participants were reimbursed for their participation.

Potential participants attended a screening interview to determine eligibility. During this interview, the screening procedure and the study were first described. After asking any questions and passing a true–false quiz that probed the candidate’s understanding of the protocol, the candidate and investigator signed the consent form in the presence of a witness. To determine eligibility, participants had a physical exam, submitted a blood and urine sample for routine testing, received an EKG, and filled out several questionnaires about their medical and drug-use history. To evaluate psychiatric status, a master’s level psychologist administered the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (SCID-DSM-IV; First et al. 1996). The study purpose was explained, and participants were told that they might receive a stimulant, sedative, antidepressant, empathogen (MDMA or MDA), or a placebo.

Participants were instructed not to take any drugs, including recreational drugs, between sessions and alcohol for 12 h before a session. Urine drug and breath alcohol tests were conducted before every session to assure that these restrictions had been followed. Participants were allowed to consume their usual amounts of caffeine and nicotine.

Procedure

The protocol consisted of two phases and utilized a placebo-controlled, within-subject crossover design. Drugs were administered under double-blind conditions, and sessions were separated by at least 48 h. Sessions were conducted in a comfortable laboratory environment and began at approximately the same time each session. One to four individuals could participate at any one time. The other individuals may or may not have been participating in the same protocol. The consumption of caffeinated beverages was not allowed during the session. Smoking was allowed except for a 30-min period before drug ingestion and 30 min before each evaluation.

Participants reported to the laboratory for a total of six sessions. As soon as they arrived, breath alcohol levels were obtained to verify that they were alcohol-free (Alco-Sensor III, Intoximeters), and urine was tested for the presence of cocaine, amphetamines, opiates, benzodiazepines, and barbiturates using fluorescence polarization immunoassay (Abbott ADx® analyzer and standard reagents). Sessions were rescheduled if the breath or urine test detected recent drug use. Pregnancy tests were also performed on urine specimens from woman participants.

The effects of placebo and 1.5 mg/kg MDMA were assessed under placebo and 20 mg/day fluoxetine maintenance conditions. The two drug administration sessions occurred between 5 and 11 days after the initiation of the maintenance condition. The order of drug administration across these two sessions was counterbalanced. At least 48 h after the second session of each maintenance condition, there was a lottery session (see description under “Multiple Choice Procedure” below). Placebo maintenance was always tested first because of the long half-life of fluoxetine. However, neither the participant nor the research staff was aware of this fixed sequence. The daily supplies of placebo and fluoxetine were given to the participant on day 1 and after each session for the days when experimental sessions were not conducted. Participants were required to telephone the laboratory every morning when they took the capsule on these nonexperimental session days.

Experimental sessions were 8 h in duration. After obtaining the battery of baseline physiological and subjective effects assessments (see below), the placebo or fluoxetine capsule was administered with water. An hour later, a second battery of assessments was made, and another capsule was administered containing MDMA or placebo. Physiological signs were assessed every 30 min for the remaining 7 h, and subjective effects were collected every hour. At the end of the session, the Multiple Choice Procedure and an End-of-Session Questionnaire were given (see below), and participants were allowed to leave if they were free of any drug effect that would interfere with functioning.

Subjective effects measures

Addiction Research Center Inventory (ARCI)

Martin et al. (1971) have compiled a shortened version (49 true–false items) from the 550-item ARCI that is separated into five scales (Amphetamine [A], Benzedrine Group [BG], Lysergic Acid Diethylamide [LSD], Morphine–Benzedrine Group [MBG], Pentobarbital–Chlorpromazine–Alcohol Group). Participants indicated by circling T for true and F for false whether the statement represented how they felt.

Profile of Mood States (POMS)

This version (modified from McNair et al. 1971) consists of 72 adjectives commonly used to describe mood states and has been factor-analyzed into eight scales (Anger, Anxiety, Confusion, Depression, Elation, Fatigue, Friendliness, Vigor). Two unvalidated scales have been derived as well (Arousal and Positive Mood). Participants indicated how they felt at the moment in relation to each of the adjectives from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely).

Visual Analog Scales (VAS)

The VAS consisted of a series of 19 horizontal 100-mm lines, each labeled with an adjective (Alert, Anxious, Bad Drug Effect, Confusion, Down, Friendly, Good Drug Effect, High, Hungry, Irritable, Liking, Miserable, On Edge, Sedated, Self-Conscience, Social, Stimulated, Talkative, Tired). Participants were instructed to place a mark on each line indicating how they felt at the moment from “not at all” to “extremely”.

Hallucinogen Rating Scale (HRS)

This scale was developed and validated by Strassman et al. (1994) for measuring hallucinogenic symptoms after dimethyltryptamine administration. The HRS identifies six domains that purportedly describe hallucinogenic experiences. Each of the items is a phrase or a single word, and participants were asked to rate how or what they felt from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). The six domains (and the number of items per domain) include: (a) somesthesia (13 items; e.g., “a rush”; “body feels different”; “feeling removed, detached, separated from body”); (b) affect (17 items; e.g., “anxious”; “awe”; “change in feelings of closeness of people in room”); (c) perception (17 items; e.g., “change in skin sensitivity”; “a sound or sounds accompanying the experiment”; “room overlaid with visual pattern”); (d) cognition (12 items; e.g., “new thoughts or insights”; “change in rate of thinking”; “change in sense of sanity”); (e) volition (eight items; e.g., “able to ‘let go’”; “able to focus attention”; “in control”; all of which are reversed scored); and (f) intensity (three items; e.g., “waxing and waning of the experience”; “intensity”; “high”). One of the items on the affect scale and six of the items on the volition scale are reversed scored. The characteristics of the questions proscribe its administration before any capsule administration.

Multiple-Choice Procedure (MCP)

The MCP (Griffiths et al. 1993) was used to assess the reinforcing effects of MDMA and placebo at the end of sessions under both maintenance conditions. Participants were required to make a series of 20 discrete choices between the capsule they received during that session vs 20 ascending values of money (from give up $5 to receive $5). The money value at which participants stop choosing drug and start choosing money is referred to as the “crossover point”. Participants were informed that one choice from the MCP (total of 40 across the two sessions) would be reinforced during session 3 or 6, which were called lottery sessions. At that time, participants were presented with a bowl containing 40 numbered poker chips; one chip corresponded with each choice between drug and money amounts on the MCP during sessions 1–2 or 4–6. The participant randomly picked one poker chip out of the bowl, and the choice indicated by the number on the chip (money or drug) was reinforced with the presentation of either the indicated amount of money (or if “give up”, that amount was subtracted from their earnings) or the indicated drug. Participants remained in the laboratory for the entire 8-h session regardless of whether they received a drug capsule or money. Vital signs were monitored hourly for safety reasons but were not used in any analysis.

End-of-Session Liking Questionnaire

At the end of every session except 3 and 6, participants were asked to indicate how much they liked the drug overall on a VAS similar to those described above [End of Session (EOS) Liking].

Data analyses

Peak drug effects were determined for the physiological and subjective effects (POMS, VAS, ARCI, and HRS) of placebo and MDMA under the two maintenance conditions. A two-way ANOVA was performed with drug (placebo or MDMA) and condition (placebo or fluoxetine maintenance) as the within-subject factors. Similarly, a two-way ANOVA was performed on the EOS Liking and MCP comparing MDMA and placebo scores under the two conditions. Post hoc analyses were done using least significant differences. For all analyses, violations of sphericity were addressed using the Huynh–Feldt adjustment factor. Significance levels were set at 0.05.

Drug supplies

Racemic MDMA HCl was obtained from David Nichols (Purdue University) in powder form so that it was possible to administer doses on a milligram-per-kilogram basis. The measured powder was placed in gelatin capsules and filled with dextrose. Placebo capsules were filled with dextrose. All capsules were identical in color and size. Dosages were based on the salt weight.

Results

Participants

A total of 10 individuals met the criteria for inclusion and participated in at least one session. Two participants dropped out after the first phase, and the remaining eight completed the study. These included six white men and two white women, and their average age was 23.9 years (range 19–33). On average, they had used MDMA 28.6 times (range 4–66). All had used other recreational drugs but for each individual sedative, opiate and stimulant drug, including cocaine, lifetime use could not have exceeded 50 times because of the exclusion criterion (Table 1).

Table 1 Percent of participants (n = 8) with lifetime or current use of recreational drugs

Subjective and physiological effects

There were several significant main drug effects (see Table 2). On the ARCI, MDMA increased scores on the Amphetamine, LSD, MBG, and BG scales and decreased scores on the PCAG scale relative to placebo. For POMS, there were significant increases for Arousal, Anxiety, Elation, Positive Mood, and Vigor. VAS ratings of Anxious, Drug Liking, Friendly, Good Drug Effect, High, Stimulated, and Talkative increased after MDMA. MDMA also increased HRS Affect, Cognition, Intensity, Perception, and Soma. Representative results are presented in Fig. 1. There were also significant drug effects on MCP crossover point and EOS Liking, and with both measures, the scores were higher for MDMA compared to placebo (Fig. 2). MDMA also increased heart rate and systolic and diastolic blood pressure (Fig. 3). In the post hoc analyses, in many cases where there was a significant main drug effect, this was due to significant differences in the effects of MDMA and placebo only when the maintenance condition was placebo (see Table 2). This was true for ARCI Amphetamine and MBG, all the POMS scales, VAS Friendly and Talkative, HRS Affect, Cognition and Soma scales, and MCP crossover point.

Table 2 Subjective and physiological effects of placebo and MDMA when maintained on placebo and fluoxetine (significant drug or condition × drug interactions*)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Peak effects of placebo (open bars) and 1.5 mg/kg MDMA (closed bars) in combination with placebo pretreatment (left-hand side) or 20 mg fluoxetine pretreatment (right-hand side) on ARCI MBG (left top panel), POMS Arousal (right top panel), VAS Drug Liking (left bottom panel), and HRS Affect (right bottom panel). See Table 2 for post hoc analysis results

Fig. 2
figure 2

The effects of placebo (open bars) and 1.5 mg/kg MDMA (closed bars) in combination with placebo pretreatment (left-hand side) or 20 mg fluoxetine pretreatment (right-hand side) on End of Session Drug Liking (left panel) and MCP Crossover point (right panel). See Table 2 for post hoc analysis results

Fig. 3
figure 3

Peak effects of placebo (open bars) and 1.5 mg/kg MDMA (closed bars) in combination with placebo pretreatment (left-hand side) or 20 mg fluoxetine pretreatment (right-hand side) on systolic blood pressure (top panel), diastolic blood pressure (middle panel), and heart rate (bottom panel). See Table 2 for post hoc analysis results

There were also several measures where there were significant drug × condition effects. The post hoc analyses revealed that maintenance on fluoxetine attenuated many of the effects of MDMA. This was not true for placebo, i.e., in no case were the effects of placebo administration different under the fluoxetine vs placebo maintenance condition. As seen in Table 2, fluoxetine decreased MDMA effects on ARCI MBG, POMS Arousal, Elation, Positive Mood and Vigor, VAS Drug Liking, High and Stimulated, HRS Affect and Soma, EOS Liking, and heart rate.

Discussions

The major finding of this study is that treatment with fluoxetine, a selective 5-HT reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), for at least 5 days attenuated the many, but not all, subjective effects of MDMA. After pretreatment with placebo, MDMA produced changes in subjective and physiological effects remarkably similar to those found in previous studies (Harris et al. 2002; Johanson et al. 2006; Liechti et al. 2000a; Tancer and Johanson 2003). Many of these subjective effects are similar to those seen after the administration of other drugs of abuse such as amphetamine and may underlie MDMA’s popularity as a recreational drug.

When fluoxetine was given for at least 5 days before the assessment of MDMA effects, many of the positive effects were attenuated. Fluoxetine itself had no effects on the measures of subjective effects as indicated by the similarity of effects of placebo under both maintenance conditions. The effects of MDMA on heart rate were also attenuated but blood pressure was not affected. It is also noteworthy that fluoxetine dampened the effects of MDMA on scales of the HRS, which have been postulated to reflect hallucinogenic-like properties. These findings are consistent with the results reported by Liechti et al. (Liechti et al. 2000a; Liechti and Vollenweider 2000b) who administered intravenous citalopram, another SSRI, and demonstrated an attenuation of a wide range of MDMA effects, both subjective and physiological. In addition, Liechti et al. (2000b) demonstrated that a 5-HT2 antagonist, ketanserin, also blocked subjective and physiological effects of MDMA in humans, implicating these receptors as important in the mediation of these effects. Like the findings in the present study, however, not all effects were blocked consistently in these two studies which, taken together, could be interpreted to mean that other systems play a role. It is interesting to note that McCann and Ricaurte (1993) published an article on antidotal reports from four individuals who claimed that ingestion of fluoxetine before MDMA use had no effects on its subjective effects.

Nevertheless, these results in humans are consistent with the findings in rats and monkeys. For instance, preclinical studies have demonstrated that SSRIs block the ability of MDMA to release 5-HT presumably because they prevent the uptake of MDMA into 5-HT neurons (e.g., Hekmatpanah and Peroutka 1990; Rudnick and Wall 1992; Schmidt and Taylor 1990). Not surprisingly, then, SSRIs have been shown to block the behavioral effects of MDMA in rats (see review by Green et al. 2003). In addition, 5-HT2 antagonists also modify the effects of MDMA. For instance, Fantegrossi et al. (2002) showed that the reinforcing effects of MDMA were attenuated in rhesus monkey given ketanserin. Taken together, the findings clearly indicate the importance of 5-HT systems in mediating the behavioral effects of MDMA in both animals and humans.

On the other hand, the failure to demonstrate complete attenuation across all measures seen in the present study and others may indicate that other neurotransmitters are also involved in the subjective and physiological effects of MDMA. For instance, there are preclinical studies demonstrating that DA plays a role in the effects of MDMA both at the molecular (Koch and Galloway 1997; Schmidt et al. 1987) and behavioral levels (Evans and Johanson 1986). An added complication is that there may be differences in neurochemical mediation as a function of the stereoisomers with the S(+)-MDMA more dopaminergic and the R(−)-MDMA more serotonergic (Baker et al. 1995; Glennon et al. 1988). Furthermore, Tancer and Johanson (2001, 2003) have demonstrated that in humans, MDMA shares many behavioral effects with d-amphetamine, and in a drug discrimination study where participants were trained to discriminate among amphetamine and a 5-HT drug, mCPP, half of the participants discriminated MDMA as amphetamine and half as mCPP (Johanson et al. 2006). However, studies in humans using DA antagonists have not only failed to block the subjective effects of classic dopaminergically mediated drugs, such as amphetamine (Brauer and de Wit 1996, 1997; Brauer et al. 1997) but in a study by Liechti and Vollenweider (2000a), haloperidol was also shown to change the subjective effects of MDMA from positive-like to more dysphoric, which they interpreted as a nonspecific interaction. Unfortunately, these results are somewhat difficult to interpret, and the role of DA in mediating the effects of both amphetamine and MDMA needs to be explored more thoroughly. Nevertheless, it seems clear that 5-HT plays a major role.

Several limitations of the study need to be acknowledged. First, the order of placebo and fluoxetine pretreatment was fixed, and the reduced effects of MDMA might be due to habituation. While this problem cannot be ruled out, in previous studies with multiple doses of MDMA, a diminution of effect over time has not been observed (Tancer and Johanson 2001, 2003). Second, only a single dose of fluoxetine was tested, and it is always possible that higher doses would be more effective. Third, in depressed individuals, 5 days of treatment with SSRIs is usually not sufficient to produce behavioral changes and thus it might be argued that there would be more complete blockade of MDMA effects if fluoxetine had been administered over a longer period of time. However, amelioration of depressive symptoms may require neuroadaptational changes, which is unlikely the case with subjective effects of MDMA. It is also interesting to note that the attenuation observed was similar to the blockade noted by Liechti et al. (2000a) after acute citalopram administration, i.e., there was no indication that a longer duration of treatment in the present study resulted in a greater degree of blockade.

In conclusion, the present study demonstrated that many of the subjective effects of MDMA as well as heart rate increases could be attenuated by fluoxetine. While the degree of blockade was not complete, these results add to the substantive literature on the role of 5-HT in the behavioral effects of MDMA in humans.