Skip to main content
Log in

Compared to Whom? An Investigation of the Relative Health Comparisons of Well People

  • Published:
Canadian Journal of Public Health Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives: Responses to the self-reported health (SRH) question, “In general how would you rate your health? Excellent, very good, good, fair or poor”, have been repeatedly demonstrated to predict mortality. Survival curves plotted for each response category show that the likelihood of death increases in a stepped fashion with each progressively negative response category and the relationship persists for up to 12 years following initial measurement. To whom do people compare themselves when answering the self-reported health question?

Methods: Twenty-one interviews with people who selected the better-health response choices (i.e., good, very good, or excellent) were conducted in a Canadian city. Qualitative content analysis was undertaken and the interviews were approached with no pre-conceived ideas about what the participants would say.

Results: Analysis of the interviews suggested that there are three key features in making a health comparison: the type (i.e., with whom), direction (i.e., upward or downward), and magnitude of the comparison (i.e., with a few people or an ideal person vs. many other people). These key features varied in a systematic way with the chosen response.

Discussion: The findings related to the direction of comparison contradict, somewhat, the theories of social psychologists but these differences may have occurred because the participants were well. Healthy people may use different factors than ill people when making social comparisons as they select a self-reported health question response category. The nature of the comparisons are complex and the responses indicate that they are affected by age, gender, and life experience.

Résumé

Objectif: Il a été prouvé à maintes reprises que les réponses déclarées par les intéressés à la question «En général, comment évalueriez-vous votre santé? (excellente, très bonne, bonne, passable ou mauvaise)» peuvent prédire la mortalité. Les courbes de survie tracées pour chaque catégorie de réponse démontrent que la probabilité de décès augmente avec chaque catégorie, et que ce rapport peut persister jusqu’à 12 ans après la mesure initiale. À qui les gens se comparent-ils lorsqu’ils répondent à cette question?

Méthode: Vingt-et-une entrevues avec des personnes ayant déclaré avoir une santé relativement bonne (c.-à-d. bonne, très bonne ou excellente) ont été menées dans une ville canadienne. Nous avons procédé à une analyse qualitative du contenu et abordé les entrevues sans avoir d’idées préconçues de ce que diraient les participants.

Résultats: L’analyse des entrevues donne à penser qu’il faut tenir compte de trois grandes caractéristiques lorsqu’on compare la santé des gens: le genre de comparaison (à qui se compare-t-on?), le sens de la comparaison (vers le haut ou vers le bas?) et l’étendue de la comparaison (par rapport à un petit nombre de personnes ou à une personne idéale, ou par rapport à beaucoup d’autres personnes?). Ces grandes caractéristiques changent systématiquement selon la réponse choisie.

Discussion: Les résultats liés au sens de la comparaison contredisent passablement les théories des psychologues sociaux, mais ces différences ont pu se produire parce que les participants allaient bien. Dans leurs réponses, les personnes en bonne santé peuvent en effet faire appel à des facteurs différents de ceux des personnes malades. La nature des comparaisons sociales est complexe, et les réponses indiquent qu’elles sont influencées par l’âge, le sexe et l’expérience de la vie.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Appels A, Bosma H, Brabauskas V, Gostautas A, Sturmans F. Self-rated health and mortality in a Lithuanian and a Dutch population. Soc Sci Med 1996;42(5):681–89.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Grant MD, Piotrowski ZH, Chappell R. Self-reported health and survival in the Longitudinal Study of Aging, 1984–1986. J Clin Epidemiol 1995;48:375–87.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Idler EL, Asngl RJ. Self-rated health and mortality in the NHANES-I epidemiologic follow-up study. Am J Public Health 1990;80:446.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Kaplan G, Barell V, Lusky A. Subjective state of health and survival in elderly adults. J Gerontol Soc Sci 1988;43(4):S114–S120.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Miilunpalo S, Vuori I, Oja P, Pasanen P, Urponen H. Self-rated health status as a health measure: The predictive value of self-reported health status on the use of physician services and on mortality in the working-age population. J Clin Epidemiol 1997;50(5):517–28.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Mossey JM, Shapiro E. Self-rated health: A predictor of mortality among the elderly. Am J Public Health 1982;72:800–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Pijls LT, Feskens EJ, Kromout D. Self-rated health, mortality, and chronic diseases in elderly men. The Zutphen Study, 1985–1990. Am J Epidemiol 1993;138:840–48.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Romelsjo A, Kaplan GA, Cohen RD, Allebeck P, Andreasson S. Protective factors and social risk factors for hospitalization and mortality among young men. Am J Epidemiol 1992;135:649–58.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Simons LA, McCallum J, Friedlander Y, Simons J. Predictors of mortality in the prospective Dubbo study of Australian elderly. Aust N Z J Med 1996;26:40–48.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Wolinsky FD, Johnson RJ. Perceived health status and mortality among older men and women. J Gerontol 1992;47(suppl):S304–S312.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Ferraro KF, Farmer MM, Wybraniec JA. Health trajectories: Long-term dynamics among black and white adults. J Health Soc Behav 1997;38:38–54.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Idler EL, Kasl SV. Self-ratings of health: Do they also predict change in functional ability? J Gerontol: Soc Sci 1995;50B(6):S344–S353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Shadbolt B. Some correlates of self-rated health for Australian women. Am J Public Health 1997;87(6):951–56.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. McCallum J, Shadbolt B, Wang D. Self-reported health and survival: A 7-year follow-up study of Australian elderly. Am J Public Health 1994;84(7):1100–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Schoenfeld DE, Malmrose LC, Blazer DG, Gold DT, Seeman TE. Self-rated health and mortality in the high-functioning elderly — a closer look at healthy individuals: MacArthur field study of successful aging. J Gerontol 1994;49(3):M109–M115.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Kaplan GA, Camacho T. Perceived health and mortality: A nine-year follow-up of the human population laboratory cohort. Am J Epidemiol 1983;117(3):292–304.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. World Health Organization. Ottawa Charter for Health Promotion. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Public Health Association, 1986.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Segovia J, Barlett RE, Edwards AC. An empirical analysis of the dimensions of health status measures. Soc Sci Med 1989;29(6):761–68.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Ratner PA, Johnson JL, Jeffery B. Examining emotional, physical, social, and spiritual health as determinants of self-rated health status. Am J Health Promot 1998;12(4):275–82.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Goldberg P, Gueguen A, Schmaus A, Nakache J-P, Goldberg M. Longitudinal study of associations between perceived health status and self reported diseases in the French Gazel cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health 2001;55:233–38.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Manor O, Matthews S, Power C. Self-rated health and limiting longstanding illness: Interrelationships with morbidity in early adulthood. Int J Epidemiol 2001;30:600–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Moum T. Self-assessed health among Norwegian adults. Soc Sci Med 1992;35:935–47.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Benyamini YB, Leventhal EA, Leventhal H. Self-assessments of health: What do people know that predicts their mortality? Res Aging 1999;21(3):477–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Pikhart H, Bobak M, Siegrist J, Pajak A, Rywik S, Kyshegyi J, et al. Psychosocial work characteristics and self-rated health in four post-communist countries. J Epidemiol Community Health 2001;55:624–30.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Kaplan GA, Goldberg DE, Everson SA, Cohen RD, Salonen R, Tuomilehto J, Salonen J. Perceived health status and morbidity and mortality: Evidence from the Kuopio Ischaemic heart disease risk factor study. Int J Epidemiol 1996;25:259–65.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. George LK, Landerman R. Health and subjective well-being: A replicated secondary data analysis. Int J Aging Hum Dev 1984;19:133–56.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Schulz R, Mittelmark MB, Kronmal R, Polak JF, Hirsch CH, German P, et al. Predictors of perceived health status in elderly men and women: The Cardiovascular Health Study. J Aging Health 1994;6(4):419–47.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. VanderZee KI, Buunk BP. Social comparison as a mediator between health problems and subjective health evaluations. Br J Soc Psychol 1995;34(1):53–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Festinger L. A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations 1954;7:117–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Wills TA. Downward comparison principles in social psychology. Psycho Bull 1981;90(2):245–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Taylor SE, Lobel M. Social comparison activity under threat: Downward evaluation and upward contacts. Psychol Rev 1989;96(4):569–75.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Buunk BP, Ybema JF. Social comparisons and occupational stress: The identification-contrast model. In: Buunk BP, Gibbons FX (Eds.), Health, Coping, and Well-being: Perspectives from Social Comparison Theory. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 1997;359–88.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Baron-Epel O, Kaplan G. General subjective health status or age-related subjective health status: Does it make a difference? Soc Sci Med 2001;53:1373–81.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Kaplan G, Baron-Epel O. What lies behind the subjective evaluation of health status? Soc Sci Med 2003;56(8):1669–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Mallinson S. Listening to respondents: A qualitative assessment of the short-form 36 health status questionnaire. Soc Sci Med 2002;54:11–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Miles MB, Huberman AM. Qualitative Data Analysis: An Expanded Sourcebook, 2nd edition. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Idler EL, Kasl S. Health perceptions and survival: Do global evaluations of health status really predict mortality? J Gerontol Soc Sci 1991;46:S55–65.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shona J. Kelly PhD.

Additional information

Sources of Funding: Dr. Kelly was supported by a CIHR/NHRDP National Health PhD Fellowship from Health Canada. Dr. Ratner was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kelly, S.J., Ratner, P.A. Compared to Whom? An Investigation of the Relative Health Comparisons of Well People. Can J Public Health 96, 462–466 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03405191

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03405191

MeSH terms

Navigation