Skip to main content
Log in

Embedding effects: Stimulus representation and response mode

  • Published:
Journal of Risk and Uncertainty Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Thecontingent valuation (CV) methodology assigns prices to environmental amenities by asking people how much they would be willing to pay in order to preserve or acquire those amenities. If this measurement procedure is valid, then responses should be sensitive to relevant changes in the amenities being judged and insensitive to irrelevant changes. One apparent demonstration of inappropriate insensitivity is theembedding effect: the observation that people are apparently willing to pay the same amount of money for a good as for a minor subset of that good. This study examined the possibility that the source of this effect lies with each of two (potentially treatable) methodological problems: 1) subjects have difficulty using quantitative (dollar) response modes to express their values; and 2) subjects have difficulty absorbing the essential details of the CV scenarios describing those goods. The study found that 1) subjects showed considerable embedding both with a simple paired-comparison response mode and with a more demanding one requiring direct dollar estimates; 2) embedding was much reduced with the simpler response mode; 3) subjects' preferences with the two response modes were usually inconsistent; 4) when asked to describe the CV scenario that they had just heard, subjects often reported key task details inaccurately; and 5) there was less embedding when tasks were reinterpreted in terms of the questions subjects reported having answered (as opposed to what had actually been asked). These results are discussed in terms of the match between the questions that investigators would like to ask and the ones that subjects are capable of answering.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Campbell, D.T., and D.W. Fiske. (1959). “Convergent and Discriminant Validation by the Multitrait-Multimatrix Method,”Psychological Bulletin 56, 81–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coombs, C. (1964).A Theory of Data. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coursey, D.L., J.L. Hovis, and W.D. Schulze. (1987). “The Disparity Between Willingness to Accept and Willingness to Pay Measures of Value,”Quarterly Journal of Economics 102, 679–690.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, R.G., D.S. Brookshire, and W.D. Schulze (eds.). (1986).Valuing Environmental Goods: An Assessment of the Contingent Valuation Method. Totowa, NJ: Rowman & Allanheld.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawes, R.M. (1972).Fundamentals of Attitude Measurement. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Desvousges, W.H., V.K. Smith, and A. Fisher. (1987). “Option Price Estimates of Water Quality Improvements: A Contingent Valuation Study for the Monongahela River,”Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 14, 248–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischhoff, B. “Insurance Decisions: A Transaction Analysis.” Submitted.

  • Fischhoff, B. (1991). “Value Elicitation: Is There Anything in There?”American Psychologist 46, 835–847.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischhoff, B., and L. Furby. (1988). “Measuring Values: A Conceptual Framework for Interpreting Transactions,”Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 1, 147–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furby, L., and B. Fischhoff. (1989). “Specifying Subjective Evaluations: A Critique of Dickie et al.'s Interpretation of their Contingent Valuation Results for Reduced Minor Health Symptoms” (US Environmental Protection Agency Cooperative Agreement #CR814655-01-0.) Eugene Research Institute, Eugene, OR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furby, L., and B. Fischhoff. (1988). “Review of Visibility Valuation Survey,” Eugene Research Institute, Eugene, OR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, G.W. (1992). “Valuing Public Goods with the Contingent Valuation Method: A Critique of Kahneman and Knetsch,”Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 22.

  • Hogarth, R. (ed.) (1982).New Directions for Methodology of Social and Behavioral Science: Question Framing and Response Consistency. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jabine, T.B., M.L. Straf, J.M. Tanur, and R. Tourangeau (eds.). (1984).Cognitive Aspects of Survey Methodology: Building a Bridge Between Disciplines. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1986). “Comments on the Contingent Valution Method.” In R.D. Cummings, D.S. Brookshire, and W.D. Schulze (eds.),Valuing Environmental Goods: An Assessment of the Contingent Valuation Method. Totowa, NJ: Rowman &Allenheld.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., and J. Knetsch. (1992). “Valuing Public Goods: The Purchase of Moral Satisfaction,”Journal of Environmental Economics & Management 22, 57–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., and A. Tversky. (1979). “Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision Under Risk,”Econometrica 47, 263–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keeney, R., and H. Raiffa. (1976).Decisions with Multiple Objectives: Preferences and Value Trade-Offs. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knetsch, J.L., and J.A. Sinden. (1987). “The Persistence of Evaluation Disparities,”Quarterly Journal of Economics 102, 691–695.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loomis, J.B. (1990). “Comparative Reliability of the Dichotomous Choice and Open-Ended Contingent Valuation Techniques,”Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 18, 78–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loomis, J.B. (1989). “Test-Retest Reliability of the Contingent Valuation Method,”American Journal of Agricultural Economics 71, 76–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Majunder, S.K., E.W. Miller, and R.R. Parizek (eds.) (1990).Water Resources in Pennsylvania: Availability, Quality, and Management. Phillipsburg, NJ: Philadelphia Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merton, R.K. (1987). “The Focussed Interview and Focus Groups,”Public Opinion Quarterly 51, 550–566.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merton, R.K., and P.I. Kendall. (1946). “The Focussed Interview,”American Journal of Sociology 51, 541–557.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R.C., and R.T. Carson. (1989).Using Surveys to Value Public Goods: The Contingent Valuation Method. Washington, D.C.: Resources for the Future.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J.C. (1978).Psychometric Theory (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, G.L., B.L. Driver, and R. Gregory (eds.). (1988).Amenity Resource Valuation: Integrating Economics with Other Disciplines. State College, PA: Venture.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poulton, E.C. (1989).Bias in Quantifying Judgments. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rae, D.A. (1984). “Benefits of Visual Air Quality in Cincinnati: Results of a Contingent Ranking Survey,” Charles River Associates, Boston, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rae, D.A. (1982). “Benefits and Costs of Improving Visibility: Case Studies of the Application of the Contingent Valuation Ranking Methodology at Mesa Verde and Great Smoky Mountain National Parks, Charles River Associates, Boston, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiling, S.D., K.J. Boyle, M.L. Phillips, and M.W. Anderson. (1990). “Temporal Reliability of Contingent Values,”Land Economics 66, 128–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., and R.L. Rosnow (eds.) (1969).Artifacts in Behavioral Research. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P., D. Griffin, and A. Tversky. (1990). “Compatibility Effects in Judgment and Choice.” In R.M. Hogarth (ed.),Insights in decision making. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P., and D. MacPhillamy. (1974). “Dimensional Commensurability and Cue Utilization in Comparative Judgment,”Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 11, 172–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, V.K. (1992). “Arbitrary Values, Good Causes, and Premature Verdicts: A Reaction to Kahneman and Knetsch,”Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 22, 71–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, V.K., and W. Desvousges. (1988).Measuring Water Quality Benefits. Boston: Kluwer-Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolley, G., et al. (1986).Establishing and Valuing the Effects of Improved Visibility in the Eastern United States. (USEPA Grant #807768-01-0.) Washington, DC: US Environmental Protection Agency.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, C.F., and E. Martin. (1984).Surveying Subjective Phenomena Vols. 1 and 2. New York: Russell Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., S. Sattath, and P. Slovic. (1988). “Contingent Weighting in Judgment and Choice,”Psychological Review 95, 371–384.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was sponsored by the Electric Power Research Institute through a subcontract with Decision Focus, Inc. Their support, encouragement, and comments are gratefully acknowledged. We greatly appreciate the help and comments of Dan Adler, Ann Bostrom, Stephanie Byram, Wendy Davis, and Claire Palmgren. Their participation at our weekly seminar made this one of the most stimulating and enjoyable projects in our collective memory. Rosa Lio Stipanovic provided essential technical support. An anonymous reviewer provided valuable insight. The views expressed are those of the authors. Marilyn Jacobs Quadrel is now with Battelle Memorial Institute Pacific Northwest Laboratories.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fischhoff, B., Quadrel, M.J., Kamlet, M. et al. Embedding effects: Stimulus representation and response mode. J Risk Uncertainty 6, 211–234 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01072612

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01072612

Key words

Navigation