Skip to main content
Log in

Requiem for the Dead Man Test?

  • Commentary
  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

    We’re sorry, something doesn't seem to be working properly.

    Please try refreshing the page. If that doesn't work, please contact support so we can address the problem.

Abstract

A popular measurement heuristic called the “Dead Man Test” predicts that behavior will be absent in vitality-challenged individuals. Unfortunately, the core idea behind the Test lacks empirical support, is hopelessly vague on several counts, and may be at odds with key aspects of behavior theory. This raises serious concerns about whether the Test should continue to be employed as a guide to behavioral measurement.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. At least not in the sense of Skinner’s (1938) objective definition: “the movement of an organism or of its parts in a frame of reference provided by the organism itself or by various external objects or fields” (p. 6).

  2. Lindsley (1991) recalled devising the test in 1965 while concerned about the target behaviors that school personnel were selecting for behavior management interventions: “If a dead boy could do it, it wasn’t behavior; we should not spend valuable school funds teaching children to play dead” (p. 457).

  3. This is an extremely serious matter given that unfalsifiable claims are one of the hallmarks of pseudoscience (Normand, 2008).

  4. Consider, for instance, the proverbial chicken running around with its head cut off, or scientific data on the continued activity of decapitated cockroaches (Choi, 2007).

  5. This poor reliability may be illustrated by the detection, through functional magnetic resonance imaging technology, of apparent neural activity in an Atlantic salmon that had been diagnosed as deceased (Bennett, Baird, Miller, & Wolford, 2011).

  6. Hypothesized categories include but are not limited to zombies, ghosts, golems, vampires, and psychological theories whose popularity is not attenuated by contrary evidence (Ferguson & Heene, 2012).

  7. A common feature of pseudoscience is presenting as “evidence” an idea’s endorsement by a famous “expert” (Hines, 1993). Lindsley certainly qualifies as an expert on applied behavioral measurement but his endorsement of the DMT does not make it valid.

References

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas S. Critchfield.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Considerations

Ethical oversight: The work does not describe primary research by the author involving either human participants or vertebrate animal subjects and therefore is not subject to oversight by a research ethics committee.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Portions of this article are based on the following non-copyrighted report: Critchfield, T.S., & Shue, E.Z.H. (in press). The Dead Man Test: A preliminary experimental analysis. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Critchfield, T.S. Requiem for the Dead Man Test?. BEHAV ANALYST 40, 539–548 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-016-0082-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-016-0082-5

Keywords

Navigation