Skip to main content
Log in

Neglected Natural Experiments Germane to the Westermarck Hypothesis

The Karo Batak and the Oneida Community

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Natural experiments wherein preferred marriage partners are co-reared play a central role in testing the Westermarck hypothesis. This paper reviews two such hitherto largely neglected experiments. The case of the Karo Batak is outlined in hopes that other scholars will procure additional information; the case of the Oneida community is examined in detail. Genealogical records reveal that, despite practicing communal child-rearing, marriages did take place within Oneida. However, when records are compared with first-person accounts, it becomes clear that, owing to age- and gender-segregating practices, most endogamously marrying individuals probably did not share a history of extensive propinquity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. One marriage occurred between individuals differing in age by 6 years; the next closest differed in age by 9 years. Following Taves’s criteria, I count these two marriages (which produced one and six offspring, respectively) in the non-cosocialized category. Because childcare was in the hands of a small number of adults, the existence of a substantial number of marriages between age-disparate community members does not challenge the Westermarck hypothesis, as the parties likely experienced limited propinquity during the childhood of the younger individual.

  2. Commenting on an earlier draft of this paper, an anonymous reviewer noted that an indirect measure of the strength of the postulated Westermarckian effects would be to compare the rate of endogamy among Oneida community members (31 out of 68 individuals who married, or 45.6%) with that characteristic of surrounding areas at the time. However, in addition to overlooking the issue of cosocialization central to Westermarck’s hypothesis, this suggestion fails to take account of the extent to which cultural differences between community members and outsiders posed a sizeable obstacle to exogamy, precluding comparisons with marital patterns in surrounding towns.

References

  • Bevc, I. (1999). The effects of early separation and intimacy on brother-sister incest. Ph.D. thesis, York University.

  • Bevc, I., & Silverman, I. (1993). Early proximity and intimacy between siblings and incestuous behavior: A test of the Westermarck theory. Ethology & Sociobiology, 14, 171–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bevc, I., & Silverman, I. (2000). Early separation and sibling incest: A test of the revised Westermarck theory. Evolution & Human Behavior, 21, 151–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carden, M. L. (1969). Oneida: Utopian community to modern corporation. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeMaria, R. (1978). Communal love at Oneida: A perfectionist vision of authority, property, and sexual order. New York: E. Mellen Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ember, M. (1975). On the origin and extension of the incest taboo. Behavior Science Research, 10, 249–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, M. T. (1993). Rethinking Oedipus: An evolutionary perspective of incest avoidance. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 411–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, M. T. (1994). “Rethinking Oedipus: An evolutionary perspective of incest avoidance”: Reply. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 297–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D. M. T., & Navarrete, C. D. (2004). Third-party attitudes toward sibling incest: Evidence for Westermarck’s hypotheses. Evolution & Human Behavior, 25, 277–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foster, W. L. (1991). Women, family, and utopia: Communal experiments of the Shakers, the Oneida community, and the Mormons. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, J. R. (1962). Sibling incest. British Journal of Sociology, 13, 128–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R. (1980). The red lamp of incest. New York: Dutton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, H. (1995). Human nature and the nature of romantic love. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Santa Barbara.

  • Hartung, J. (1985). Review of “Incest: A biosocial view,” by J. Shepher. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 67, 169–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jankowiak, W. R., & Fischer, E. F. (1998). A cross-cultural perspective on romantic love. In J. M. Jenkins, K. Oatley, et al. (Eds.), Human emotions: A reader (pp. 55–62). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kipp, R. S. (1986). Terms of endearment: Karo Batak lovers as siblings. American Ethnologist, 13, 632–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, D., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2003). Does morality have a biological basis? An empirical test of the factors governing moral sentiments relating to incest. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 270, 819–826.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, D., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2007). The architecture of kin human detection. Nature, 445, 727–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, J. (1983). FBD marriage: Further support for the Westermarck hypothesis of the incest taboo? American Anthropologist, 85, 50–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, J. (1985). FBD marriage, Westermarck, and incest taboos: Replies to Dodd and Prothro, Graber, and Kopytoff. American Anthropologist, 87, 135–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neubauer, P. B. (Ed.) (1965). Children in collectives: Child-rearing aims and practices in the kibbutz. Springfield, IL: C. C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nordhoff, C. (1965). The communistic societies of the United States; from personal visit and observation including detailed accounts of the Economists, Zoarites, Shakers, the Amana, Oneida, Bethel, Aurora, Icarian, and other existing societies, their religious creeds, social practices, numbers, industries, and present condition. New York: Schocken Books (originally published in 1875).

    Google Scholar 

  • Noyes, P. B. (1937). My father’s house: An Oneida boyhood. New York: Farrar & Rinehart.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parker, H., & Parker, S. (1986). Father-daughter sexual abuse: An emerging perspective. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 531–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, C. N. (Ed.) (1970). Oneida community: An autobiography, 1851–1876. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, C. N. (Ed.) (1972). Oneida community: The breakup, 1876–1881. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepher, J. (1971). Mate selection among second generation kibbutz adolescents and adults: Incest avoidance and negative imprinting. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 1, 293–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepher, J. (1983). Incest: A biosocial view. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singarimbun, M. (1975). Kinship, descent, and alliance among the Karo Batak. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, M. E. (1958). Children of the kibbutz. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, M. E. (1979). Gender and culture: Kibbutz women revisited. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmon, Y. (1964). Mate selection in collective settlements. American Sociological Review, 29, 491–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taves, D. R. (1994). “Rethinking Oedipus: An evolutionary perspective of incest avoidance”: Comment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teeple, J. B. (1985). The Oneida family: Genealogy of a 19th century perfectionist commune, containing original community photographs and drawings. Oneida, NY: Oneida Community Historical Committee.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Berghe, P. L. (1983). Human inbreeding avoidance: Culture in nature. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 6, 91–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, A. (1997). The evolutionary psychology of mate selection in Morocco: A multivariate analysis. Human Nature, 8, 113–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, A., & Buyske, S. (2003). The Westermarck effect and early childhood co-socialization: Sex differences in inbreeding-avoidance. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 353–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westermarck, E. (1891). The history of human marriage. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, L. M., & Finkelhor, D. (1995). Paternal caregiving and incest: Test of a biosocial model. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 65, 101–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, A. P. (1993). Westermarck redivivus. Annual Review of Anthropology, 22, 157–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, A. P. (1995). Sexual attraction and childhood association: A Chinese brief for Edward Westermarck. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, A. P. (2003). Maternal sentiments: How strong are they? Current Anthropology, 44, s31–s40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I thank Joan Silk for encouragement, Jennifer Fessler for stimulating discussions, and four anonymous reviewers for helpful feedback.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel M. T. Fessler.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fessler, D.M.T. Neglected Natural Experiments Germane to the Westermarck Hypothesis. Hum Nat 18, 355–364 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-007-9021-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-007-9021-1

Keywords

Navigation