Skip to main content
Log in

Longitudinal course of rapid naming in disabled and nondisabled readers

  • Part II Consequences And Correlates Of Dyslexia: Beyond Phoneme Awareness
  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Rapid Automatized Naming Test (Denckla and Rudel 1974) was studied cross-sectionally in an sample of kindergartners (n=342) at-risk for reading disability (Study 1), and longitudinally in an n=160 epidemiological normal sample of children tested in first, third, fifth, and eighth grades (Study 2). Study 1 showed faster absolute naming speeds for those with near perfect untimed alphabet recitation, but the stronger and more orderly relation (at r=.31, p<.0001) was between three levels of alphabet recitation accuracy and the relative number/letter naming speed advantage (ratio of mean number/letter naming speed minus mean color/object naming speed over mean color/object naming speed). In Study 2, the number/letter advantage was already strongly present by first grade, and did not increase significantly thereafter, but absolute naming times improved steadily across grades in an exponential decay function. In this sample, the relative number/letter advantage was not related to reading level. However, the absolute color/object naming speed was strongly related to reading level and vocabulary across grades. Norms for the Rapid Automatized Naming Test based on the epidemiological normal sample tested in Grades 1, 3, 5, and 8 are presented in the appendix.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackerman, P. T., and Dykman, R. A. 1993. Phonological processes, confrontational naming, and immediate memory in dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities 26(9):597–609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M. J. 1990. Beginning to Read. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A. 1993. Phonemic awareness, naming, visual symbol processing, and reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 5:87–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A. 1995. Predicting reading ability over the long term: The changing roles of letter naming, phonological awareness and orthographic processing. Annals of Dyslexia 45:79–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A., Duffy, F. H., Als, H., and McAnulty, G. B. 1991. Linguistic profiles of dyslexics and good readers. Annals of Dyslexia 41:221–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A., McAnulty, G. B., Duffy, F.H., and Als, H. 1990. Prediction of dyslexia in kindergarten boys. Annals of Dyslexia 40:152–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I. L., and Juel, C. 1995. The role of decoding in learning to read. American Educator 19: 8–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biddle, K. R. 1996. Timing deficits in impaired readers: an investigation of visual naming speed and verbal fluency. Ph.D. Diss., Tufts University, Boston.

  • Blachman, B. 1984. Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills in kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology 76(4):610–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., Steffy, R., and Tate, E. 1988. Comparison of the effects of IQ control methods and naming speed predictors of reading disability. Reading Research Quarterly 23:304–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G. 1993. Text reading rereading: Predictors of fluency beyond word recognition. Journal of Reading Behavior 25:133–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., and Wolf, M. 1993. Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 5:69–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., and Swanson, L. B. 1991. Naming speed deficits in reading disability: Multiple measures of a singular process. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 51:195–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornwall, A. 1992. The relationship of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and verbal memory to severe reading and spelling disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities 25:532–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., and Stanovich, K. E. 1990. Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology 82(4):733–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., and Rudel, R. G. 1974. Rapid automatized naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex 10:86–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M., and Rudel, R. G. 1976. Rapid automatized naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia 14:471–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., and Dunn, L. M. 1981. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fawcett, A., and Nicholson, R. 1994. Naming speed in children with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities 27(10):641–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Felton, R. H., and Brown, I. S. 1990. Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2:39–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Felton, R. H., Naylor, C. E., and Wood, F. B. 1990. Neuropsychological profile of adult dyslexics. Brain and Language 39:485–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Felton, R.H. 1992. Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities. Topics in Exceptional Children and Special Education 12(2):212–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flowers, D. L. 1993. Biological basis for reading disability: A review of work in progress. Journal of Learning Disabilities 26:575–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W. 1987. A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed criteria of normal and deficient reading skill. Child Development 58: 234–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, G. R., 1995. Toward a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia 45:3–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A., and Kaurman, N. 1990. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Korhonen, T. 1995. The persistence of rapid naming problems in children with reading disability: A nine year follow-up. Journal of Learning Disabilities 28(4):232–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., and Samuels, J. 1974. Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology 6:293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathes, P. G., Simmons, D. C., and Davis, B. I. 1992. Assisted reading techniques for developing reading fluency. Reading, Research, and Instruction 31(4):70–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, M. S., Wood, F. B., Hart, L., and Felton, R. H. 1998. Selective predictive value of rapid automatized naming within poor readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities 31(2):106–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., Forsberg, H., Wise, B., and Rack, J. 1994. Measurement of word recognition, orthographic and phonological skills. In G.R. Lyon, ed., Frames of Reference for the Assessment of Learning Disabilities: New Views on Assessment Issues. Baltimore: Paul Brooks Publishing: 243–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Otis, A.S., and Lennon, R. T. 1968. Otis-Lennon Mental Ability Test. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C., Beck, I., Bell, L., and Hughes, C. 1987. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 33(3):283–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porter, R., and Hughes, L. 1983. Dichotic Listening to CV’s: Method, Interpretation, and Application. In J. Hellige, ed., Cerebral Hemispheric Asymmetry: Method, Theory, and Application. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J., Court, J., and Raven, J. 1984 Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H.K Lewis & Co. Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, E., and DiBenedetto, B. 1985. Decoding Skills Test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, S. J. 1987. Information processing abilities and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities 20(1):18–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., and Liberman, I. Y. 1989. Phonology and Reading Disability. Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, L. S., and Downey, D. M. 1995. The serial rapid naming skills of reading disabled children. Annals of Dyslexia 45:31–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. 1986. Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly 21:360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. 1988. Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities 21:590–612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., and Rashotte, C. A. 1994. Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities 27:276–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Simmons, K., and Laughon, P. 1990 Identifying phonological coding problems in disabled readers: Naming, counting and span measures? Learning Disabilities Quarterly 13: 236–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., and Torgesen, J. K. 1987. The nature of phonological processing and its casual role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin 101:192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, D. J., Price, G., and Gillingham, M. G. 1988. The critical but transitory importance of letter naming. Reading Research Quarterly 23:108–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bally, H., and Morris, R. 1986. Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development 57:988–1000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. 1990. The word retrieval deficit hypothesis and developmental dyslexia. Presented at American Educational Association Meetings, Boston, Mass.

  • Wolf, M. 1991. Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly 26:123–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., and Bowers, P. 1997. The “double-deficit hypothesis” for the developmental dyslexias. Unpublished paper, submitted for publication.

  • Wolf, M., and Obregon, M. 1997. The “double-deficit” hypothesis: Implications for diagnosis and practice in reading disabilities. In L. Putman, ed., Readings on Language and Literacy. Cambridge, MA: Brookline Books, 177–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W., and Johnson, M.B. 1977. Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-educational Battery. Allen, TX: DLM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, A., Bowers, P., and MacKinnon, G. 1996. Effects of prosodic modeling and repeated reading on poor readers’ fluency and comprehension. Applied Psycholinguistics 17: 59–84.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marianne S. Meyer.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Meyer, M.S., Wood, F.B., Hart, L.A. et al. Longitudinal course of rapid naming in disabled and nondisabled readers. Ann. of Dyslexia 48, 89–114 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-998-0005-6

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-998-0005-6

Keywords

Navigation