Skip to main content
Log in

Phonological processing and emergent literacy in Spanish-speaking preschool children

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Phonological awareness (PA), phonological memory (PM), and phonological access to lexical storage (also known as RAN), play important roles in acquiring literacy. We examined the convergent, discriminant, and predictive validity of these phonological processing abilities (PPAs) in 147 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children whose native language was Spanish. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) supported the validity of each PPA as separate from general cognitive ability and separate from each other. Moreover, structural equation modeling found RAN uniquely associated with knowledge of Spanish letter names and Spanish letter sounds. PA was found the best predictor of children’s ability to distinguish alphabetic text from nonalphabetic text. Finally, general cognitive ability was only indirectly associated with emergent literacy skills via PPAs. These results highlight the importance of PPAs in the early literacy development of native Spanish speaking preschool children.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adrian, J. A., Alegria, J., & Morais, J. (1995). Metaphonological abilities of Spanish illiterate adults. International Journal of Psychology, 30, 329–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alegria, J., Pignot, E., & Morais, J. (1982). Phonetic analysis of speech and memory codes in beginning readers. Memory and Cognition, 10, 451–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, J. L., & Assel, M. A. (in press). A first look at the validity of the Spanish version of the DIAL-3. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment.

  • Anthony, J. L., & Francis, D. J. (2005). Development of phonological awareness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 255–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, J. L., & Lonigan, C. J. (2004). The nature of phonological sensitivity: Converging evidence from four studies of preschool and early grade-school children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 43–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, J. L., Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., Driscoll Bacon, K., Phillips, B. M., & Cantor, B. G. (2002). Structure of preschool phonological sensitivity: Overlapping sensitivity to rhyme, words, syllables, and phonemes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 65–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, J. L., Lonigan, C. J., Driscoll, K., Phillips, B. M., & Burgess, S. R. (2003). Phonological sensitivity: A quasi-parallel progression of word structure units and cognitive operations. Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 470–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, J. L., Williams, J. M., McDonald, R., Corbitt-Shindler, D., & Francis, D. J. (2006). Phonological representation, access, memory and awareness: The structure of phonological processing abilities in English speaking preschool children. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Barth, A. E. (2006). An investigation of the reading-related component skills that underly reading fluency of adolescent readers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Kansas.

  • Bentin, S., Hammer, R., & Cahan, S. (1991). The effects of aging and first grade schooling on the development of phonological awareness. Psychological Science, 2, 271–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertelson, P., de Gelder, B., Tfouni, L.V., & Morais, J. (1989). Metaphonological abilities of adult illiterates: New evidence of heterogeneity. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 1, 239–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S., Fowler, A., Stone, B., & Winbury, N. (1994). Training phonological awareness: A study with inner-city kindergarten children. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 26–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E., Bradley, L., MacLean, M., & Crossland, J. (1989). Nursery rhymes, phonological skills and reading. Journal of Child Language, 16, 407–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection, and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, S. R., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Bidirectional relations of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a preschool sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 117–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardoso-Martins, C. (1991). Awareness of phonemes and alphabetic literacy acquisition. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 61, 164–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carrillo, M. (1994). Development of phonological awarness and reading acquisition: A study in Spanish language. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 279–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities. Cambridge, UK:: University of Cambridge Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cisero, C. A., & Royer, J. M. (1995). The development and cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, 275–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Comeau, L., Cormier, P., Grandmaison, E., & Lacroix, D. (1999). A longitudinal study of phonological processing skills in children learning to read in a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 29–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., Katz, L., & Tola, G., (1988). Awarness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P., & van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Manrique, A. M. B., & Signorini, A. (1998). Emergent writing forms in Spanish. Reading and Writing, 10, 499–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Felton, R. H., & Brown, I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 39–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gombert, J. E. (1994). How do illiterate adults react to metalinguistic training? Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 250–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, P. (1991). Word-finding ability and design fluency in developmental dyslexia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 30, 47–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoskyn, M., & Swanson, L. (2000). Cognitive processing of low achievers and children with reading disabilities: A selective meta-analytic review of the published literature. School Psychology Review, 29, 102–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jamshidian, M., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). ML estimation of mean and covariance structures with missing data using complete data routines. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 24, 21–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jimenez, J. E. (1997). A reading-level match study of phonemic processes underlying reading disabilities in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 9, 23–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jimenez, J. E., Garcia, E., Ortiz, R., Hernandez-Valle, I., Guzman, R., Rodrigo, M., Estevez, A., Diaz, A., & Hernandez, S. (2005). Is the deficit in phonological awareness better explained in terms of task differences or effects of syllable structure? Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 267–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jimenez, J. E., & Valle, I. H. (2000). Word identification and reading disorders in the Spanish language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 44–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamhi, A., & Catts, H. (1989). Reading disabilities: A developmental language perspective. Boston: College-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsey, K. A., Manis, F. R., & Bailey, C. E. (2003). Prediction of first-grade reading in Spanish-speaking English-language learners. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 482–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., Anthony, J. L., & Barker, T. A. (1998). Development of phonological sensitivity in two- to five-year-old children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 294–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J., & Farver, J. M. (2002). Spanish preschool comprehensive test of phonological and print processing. Unpublished test. Florida State University.

  • Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Petersen, O. P. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Olofsson, A., & Wall, S. (1980). Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21, 159–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Lindsey, K. A., & Bailey, C. E. (2004). Development of reading in grades K-2 in Spanish-speaking English-language learners. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 19, 214–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mardell-Czudnowski, C., & Goldenberg, D. S. (1998). Developmental indicators for the assessment of learning-third edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naslund, J. C. (1990). The interrelationships among preschool predictors of reading acquisition for German children. Reading and Writing, 2, 327–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., Halpern, D. F., Loehlin, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., & Urbina, S. (1996). Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L. C., & Hughes, C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 283–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapala, M. M., & Brady, S. (1990). Reading ability and short-term memory: The role of phonological processing. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohl, M., & Pratt, C. (1995). Phonological awareness, verbal working memory and the acquisition of literacy. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 327–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., Katz, L., Fowler, A. E., Liberman, A. M., Brady, S. A., Thornton, R., Lundquist, E., Dreyer, L., Fletcher, J. M., Stuebing, K. K., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1995). Cognitive profiles of reading-disabled children: Comparison of language skills in phonology, morphology, and syntax. Psychological Science, 6, 149–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., Goulandris, N., Bowlby, M., & Howell, P. (1986). Segmentation and speech perception in relation to reading skill: A developmental analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 489–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprugevica, I., & Høien, T. (2004). Relations between enabling skills and reading comprehension: A follow-up study of Latvian students from first to second grade. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 45, 115–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 590–612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, B., & Brady, S. (1995). Evidence for deficits in basic phonological processes in less-skilled readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 51–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. H., Saez, L., Gerber, M., & Leafsteadt, J. (2004). Literacy and cognitive functioning in bilingual and nonbilingual children at or not at risk for reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 3–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vukovic, R. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2006). The double-deficit hypothesis: A comprehensive analysis of the evidence. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 25–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Balthazor, M., Hurley, S., Morgan, S., Rashotte, C. A., Shaner, R., Simmons, K., & Stage, S. (1987). The nature of prereaders’ phonological-processing abilities. Cognitive Development, 2, 355–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmons, K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1993). Development of young readers’ phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 83–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., Donahue, J., & Garon, T. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Obregon, M. (1992). Early naming deficits, developmental dyslexia, and a specific deficit hypothesis. Brain and Language, 42, 219–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W., & Muñoz-Sandoval, A. F. (1993). Batería Woodcock-Muñoz Revisada. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. C. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia and skilled reading across languages. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jason L. Anthony.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Anthony, J.L., Williams, J.M., McDonald, R. et al. Phonological processing and emergent literacy in Spanish-speaking preschool children. Ann. of Dyslexia 56, 239–270 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-006-0011-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-006-0011-5

Key Words

Navigation