Skip to main content
Log in

Study techniques differentially influence the delayed judgment-of-learning accuracy of adolescent children and college-aged adults

  • Published:
Metacognition and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The ways in which adult learners study information influences their judgment-of-learning (JOL) accuracy (e.g., Koriat et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(2), 147-162, 2002). The present study extends this investigation to adolescent children to determine whether developing learners’ metacognitive monitoring is similarly influenced by different study techniques. In two experiments, we examined JOL accuracy in adolescent children (ages 11 to 12) and college-aged adults. Across both experiments, we employed a standard delayed-JOL paradigm in which three groups of participants, differing in type of study technique, encoded weakly-related word pairs. One group studied the word pairs twice (study practice). A second group studied with the instruction to generate a word that linked the two members of the word pair together (elaborative encoding). The final group studied word pairs and then took an immediate cued-recall test with feedback (retrieval practice). In children and adults, retrieval practice led to better JOL accuracy as compared to study practice. Children differed from adults in how elaborative encoding influenced JOL accuracy. For adults, elaborative encoding resulted in better JOL accuracy than study practice; however, for children, JOL accuracy did not differ between the two groups. Our results suggest that encoding processes influence delayed-JOL accuracy in both age-groups.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Begg, I., Duft, S., Lalonde, P., Melnick, R., & Sanvito, J. (1989). Memory predictions are based on ease of processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 28(5), 610–632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beuhring, T., & Kee, D. W. (1987a). Elaborative Propensities During Adolescence: The Relationships Among Memory Knowledge, Strategy Behavior, and Memory Performance. In M. A. McDaniel & M. Pressley (Eds.), Imagery and Related Mnemonic Processes. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beuhring, T., & Kee, D. W. (1987b). Developmental relationships among metamemory, elaborative strategy use, and associative memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 44(3), 377–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, G. A., Marsh, R. L., Clark-Foos, A., & Meeks, J. T. (2010). Noncriterial recollection influences metacognitive monitoring and control processes. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(10), 1936–1942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K. (2009). Cue strength as a moderator of the testing effect: The benefits of elaborative retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(6), 1563–1569.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K. (2011). Semantic information activated during retrieval contributes to later retention: Support for the mediator effectiveness hypothesis of the testing effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(6), 1547–1552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, G. I., Marsh, R. L., & Hicks, J. L. (2006). Source memory in the absence of successful cued recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(4), 828–835.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, K. A., Toth, J. P., & Hertzog, C. (2009). Aging and recollection in the accuracy of judgments of learning. Psychology and Aging, 24(2), 494–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Nelson, T. O. (1994). Does the sensitivity of judgments of learning (JOLs) to the effects of various study activities depend on when the JOLs occur? Journal of Memory and Language, 33(4), 545–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., Hertzog, C., & Powell-Moman, A. (2005). The contribution of mediator-based deficiencies to age differences in associative learning. Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 389–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finn, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2014). Overconfidence in children's multi-trial judgments of learning. Learning and Instruction, 32, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., Fulton, E. K., Sinclair, S. M., & Dunlosky, J. (2014). Recalled aspects of original encoding strategies influence episodic feelings of knowing. Memory & Cognition, 42(1), 126–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann-Biencourt, A., Lockl, K., Schneider, W., Ackerman, R., & Koriat, A. (2010). Self-pace study time as a cue for recall predictions across school age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 767–784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isingrini, M., Sacher, M., Perrotin, A., Taconnat, L., Souchay, C., Stoehr, H., & Bouazzaoui, B. (2016). Episodic feeling-of-knowing relies on noncriterial recollection and familiarity: Evidence using an online remember-know procedure. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 41, 31–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jang, Y., Wallsten, T. S., & Huber, D. E. (2012). A stochastic detection and retrieval model for the study of metacognition. Psychological Review, 119(1), 186–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpicke, J. D. (2009). Metacognitive control and strategy selection: Deciding to practice retrieval during learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(4), 469–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, J. F., Zechmeister, E. B., & Shaughnessy, J. J. (1980). Judgments of knowing: The influence of retrieval practice. The American Journal of Psychology, 93(2), 329–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one's own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(4), 349–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Shitzer-Reichert, R. (2002). Metacognitive judgments and their accuracy. In P. Chambres, M. Izaute, & P. Marescaux (Eds.), Metacognition: Process, function and use (pp. 1–17). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Sheffer, L., & Ma'ayan, H. (2002). Comparing objective and subjective learning curves: Judgments of learning exhibit increased underconfidence with practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(2), 147–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Ma'ayan, H., Sheffer, L., & Bjork, R. A. (2006). Exploring a mnemonic debiasing account of the underconfidence-with-practice effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(3), 595–608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Ackerman, R., Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). The easily learned, easily remembered heuristic in children. Cognitive Development, 24(2), 169–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehman, M., Smith, M. A., & Karpicke, J. D. (2014). Toward an episodic context account of retrieval-based learning: Dissociating retrieval practice and elaboration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(6), 1787–1794.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonesio, R. J., & Nelson, T. O. (1990). Do different metamemory judgments tap the same underlying aspects of memory? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(3), 464–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovelace, E. A. (1984). Metamemory: Monitoring future recallability during study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(4), 756–766.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazzoni, G., Cornoldi, C., & Marchitelli, G. (1990). Do memorability ratings affect study-time allocation? Memory & Cognition, 18(2), 196–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, D. P., & Soderstrom, N. C. (2011). Recollection-based prospective metamemory judgments are more accurate than those based on confidence: Judgments of remembering and knowing (JORKS). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 605–621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meeter, M., & Nelson, T. O. (2003). Multiple study trials and judgments of learning. Acta Psychologica, 113(2), 123–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognitive judgments and control of study. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(3), 159–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Finn, B. (2013). Metacognition and control of study choice in children. Metacognition and Learning, 8(1), 19–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Dunlosky, J. (1991). When people's judgments of learning (JOLs) are extremely accurate at predicting subsequent recall: The "delayed-JOL effect.". Psychological Science, 2(4), 267–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 26, pp. 125–173). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1994). Why investigate metacognition? In J. Metcalfe & A. P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 1–25, Chapter xiii, 334 Pages)). Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (1998). The University of South Florida Word Association, Rhyme, and Word Fragment Norms.

  • Nelson, T. O., Narens, L., & Dunlosky, J. (2004). A revised methodology for research on metamemory: Pre-judgment recall and monitoring (PRAM). Psychological Methods, 9(1), 53–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parks, C. M. (2007). The role of noncriterial recollection in estimating recollection and familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(1), 81–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peynircioğlu, Z. F., Brandler, B. J., Hohman, T. J., & Knutson, N. (2014). Metacognitive judgments in music performance. Psychology of Music, 42(5), 748–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., Levin, J. R., & Ghatala, E. S. (1984). Memory strategy monitoring in adults and children. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 23(2), 270–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pyc, M. A., & Rawson, K. A. (2010). Why testing improves memory: Mediator effectiveness hypothesis. Science, 330(6002), 1–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pyc, M. A., Rawson, K. A., & Aschenbrenner, A. J. (2014). Metacognitive monitoring during criterion learning: When and why are judgments accurate? Memory & Cognition, 42(6), 886–897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. G. (2016). Judgments of learning: Methods, data, and theory. In J. Dunlosky & S. K. Tauber (Eds.), The oxford handbook of metamemory (pp. 65–80). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. G., & Tauber, S. K. (2011). The influence of delaying judgments of learning on metacognitive accuracy: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 137(1), 131–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., von der Linden, N., Schneider, W., & Howie, P. (2007). Children's metamemorial judgments in an event recall task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97(2), 117–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Visé, M., Lockl, K., & Nelson, T. O. (2000). Developmental trends in children’s monitoring: Evidence from a judgment-of-learning. Cognitive Development, 15, 115–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime: User's guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L., & Metcalfe, J. (1994). Methodological problems and pitfalls in the study of human metacognition. In J. Metcalfe & A. P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 93–113, Chapter xiii, 334 Pages)). Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaughnessy, J. J., & Zechmeister, E. B. (1992). Memory-monitoring accuracy as influenced by the distribution of retrieval practice. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30(2), 125–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W. (1999). The importance of monitoring and self-regulation during multitrial learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6(4), 662–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., & Dunlosky, J. (1994). Delaying students' metacognitive monitoring improves their accuracy in predicting their recognition performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(2), 290–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Anderson, M. C. M., & Therriault, D. (2003). Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 66–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2007). Metacomprehension for educationally relevant materials: Dramatic effects of encoding-retrieval interactions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 212–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A. K., Bulevich, J. B., & Dubois, S. J. (2011). Context affects feeling-of-knowing accuracy in younger and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(1), 96–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A. K., Bulevich, J. B., & Dubois, S. J. (2012). An analysis of the determinants of the feeling of knowing. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 21(4), 1681–1694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsalas, N., Paulus, M., & Sodian, B. (2015). Developmental changes and the effect of self-generated feedback in metacognitive controlled spacing strategies in 7-year-olds, 10-year-olds, and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 140–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tullis, J. G., Finley, J. R., & Benjamin, A. S. (2013). Metacognition of the testing effect: Guiding learners to predict the benefits of retrieval. Memory & Cognition, 41(3), 429–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Undorf, M., Böhm, S., & Cüpper, L. (2016). Do judgments of learning predict automatic influences of memory? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(6), 882–896.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Loon, M. H., de Bruin, A. B. H., van Gog, T., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2013). The effect of delayed-JOLs and sentence generation on children’s monitoring accuracy and regulation of idiom study. Metacognition and Learning, 8(2), 173–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Loon, M., de Bruin, A., Leppink, J., & Roebers, C. (2017). Why are children overconfident? developmental differences in the implementation of accessibility cues when judging concept learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 158, 77–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von der Linden, N., Löffler, E., & Schneider, W. (2015). Effects of a short strategy training on metacognitive monitoring across the life-span. Frontline Learning Research, 3(4), 36–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weaver, C. A., & Kelemen, W. L. (2003). Processing similarity does not improve metamemory: Evidence against transfer-appropriate monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(6), 1058–1065.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiffen, J. W., & Karpicke, J. D. (2017). The role of episodic context in retrieval practice effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(7), 1036–1046.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank John Kitzmiller, a mathematics educator at the Frances C. Richmond Middle School at Hanover, NH.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gregory I. Hughes.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hughes, G.I., Taylor, H.A. & Thomas, A.K. Study techniques differentially influence the delayed judgment-of-learning accuracy of adolescent children and college-aged adults. Metacognition Learning 13, 109–126 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-018-9180-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-018-9180-y

Keywords

Navigation