Skip to main content
Log in

Metasearch accuracy for letters and symbols: do our intuitions match empirical reality?

  • Published:
Metacognition and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The “familiarity effect” (Shen and Reingold, Perception & Psychophysics 63(3):464–475, 2001) is a phenomenon in which unfamiliar symbols perceptually “pop-out” when placed among familiar symbols (e.g., letters). In contrast, searching for familiar symbols among unfamiliar symbols is more challenging. Failure to account for effects such as these when predicting search performance could lead to overconfidence and error. This study investigated metacognitive awareness of the familiarity effect by asking participants to rate the speed and accuracy of search before they searched for either letter or symbol targets among letter or symbol distractors. Feature overlap between target and distractor and target presence or absence were also manipulated to provide concurrent cues to task difficulty. This study examined metacognitive awareness of the “familiarity effect,” and extended the findings from an earlier metasearch study (Redford et al., Memory and Cognition 39:1534–1545, 2011). Metacognition was accurate with respect to the familiarity effect. However, participants incorrectly predicted that they would detect a target’s absence faster than its presence. These findings suggest that people have metacognitive awareness for some aspects of visual search, even when patterns of search performance are complex and potentially counterintuitive. However, limitations exist in our metacognitive awareness of visual search. The results are discussed in relation to Koriat’s cue utilization framework and heuristic-based metacognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baranski, J. V., & Petrusic, W. M. (1994). The calibration and resolution of confidence in perceptual judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 412–428. doi:10.3758/BF03205299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (1983). Category effects in visual search: a failure to replicate the “oh-zero” phenomenon. Perception & Psychophysics, 34(3), 221–232. doi:10.3758/BF03202949.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Nelson, T. (1992). Importance of the kind of cue for judgments of learning (JOL) and the delayed-JOL effect. Memory and Cognition, 20(4), 374–380. doi:10.3758/BF03210921.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–911. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graber, M. (2005). Diagnostic errors in medicine: a case of neglect. Joint Commission Journal on Quality and Patient Safety, 31(2), 106–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, V., Monzote, L., Van den Sande, B., Mukadi, P., Sopheak, T., Gillet, P., & Jacobs, J. (2011). Assessment of the knowledge of graphical symbols labelled on malaria rapid diagnostic tests in four international settings. Malaria Journal, 10(331), 10–1186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., Touron, D. R., & Hines, J. C. (2007). Does a time-monitoring deficit influence older adults’ delayed retrieval shift during skill acquisition? Psychology and Aging, 22, 607–624. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.22.3.607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., Hines, J. C., & Touron, D. R. (2013). Judgments of learning are influenced by multiple cues in addition to memory for past test accuracy. Archives of Scientific Psychology, 1(1), 23–32. doi:10.1037/arc0000003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J., & Gleitman, H. (1972). A conceptual category effect in visual search: O as letter or as digit. Perception & Psychophysics, 12(6), 457–460. doi:10.3758/BF03210934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpicke, J. D. (2009). Metacognitive control and strategy selection: deciding to practice retrieval during learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(4), 469–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kepecs, A., & Mainen, Z. F. (2012). A computational framework for the study of confidence in humans and animals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 367, 1322–1337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D. (2005). Metacognition, distributed cognition and visual design. In P. Gardenfors & P. Johansson (Eds.), Cognition, education and communication technology (pp. 147–180). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: a cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(4), 349–370. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Bjork, R. A. (2005). Illusions of competence in monitoring one’s knowledge during study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(2), 187–194. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.2.187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, L. E. (1984). The category effect in visual search depends on physical rather than conceptual differences. Perception & Psychophysics, 35(6), 558–564. doi:10.3758/BF03205953.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kundel, H. L., & La Follette, P. S., Jr. (1972). Visual search patterns and experience with radiological images 1. Radiology, 103(3), 523–528. doi:10.1148/103.3.523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1999). Inattentional blindness. Psyche, 5(3).

  • McCarley, J. S., & Gosney, J. (2005). Metacognitive judgments in a simulated luggage screening task. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 49(17), 1620–1624. doi:10.1177/154193120504901726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., Scwartz, B. L., & Joaquim, S. G. (1993). The cue-familiarity heuristic in metacognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(4), 851–861.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T., & Dunlosky, J. (1991). When people’s judgments of learning (JOL) are extremely accurate at predicting subsequent recall: the delayed-JOL effect. Psychological Science, 2(4), 267–270. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 849–860. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.849.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redford, J. S. (2010). Evidence of metacognitive control by humans and monkeys in a perceptual categorization task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(1), 248–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redford, J. S., Green, S., Geer, M., Humphrey, M., & Thiede, K. W. (2011). Exploring metacognitive accuracy in visual search. Memory and Cognition, 39, 1534–1545. doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0123-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A. (2006). Attention, consciousness and data display. In 2006 Proceedings of the American Statistical Association, Statistical Graphics Section. Alexandria: American Statistical Association.

  • Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: the need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8(5), 368–373. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. G., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Memory predictions are influenced by perceptual information: evidence for metacognitive illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(4), 615–625. doi:10.1037/a0013684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roy, M. M., & Christenfeld, N. J. S. (2007). Bias in memory predicts bias in estimation of future task duration. Memory and Cognition, 35(2), 557–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Serra, M. J., & Dunlosky, J. (2010). Metacomprehension judgements reflect the belief that diagrams improve learning from text. Memory, 18(7), 698–711. doi:10.1080/09658211.2010.506441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shen, J., & Reingold, E. M. (2001). Visual search asymmetry: the influence of stimulus familiarity and low-level features. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(3), 464–475. doi:10.3758/BF03194413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 1059–1074. doi:10.1068/p2952.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. D., Redford, J. S., Washburn, D. A., & Taglialatela, L. A. (2005). Specific-token effects in screening tasks: possible implications for aviation security. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(6), 1175–1185. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Song, C., Kanai, R., Fleming, S. M., Weil, R. S., Schwarzkopf, D. S., & Rees, G. (2011). Relating inter-individual differences in metacognitive performance on different perceptual tasks. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1787–1792. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.12.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1967). Two operations in character recognition: some evidence from reaction-time measurements. Perception & Psychophysics, 2(2), 45–53. doi:10.3758/BF03212460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szalma, J. L., Hancock, P. A., Dember, W. N., & Warm, J. S. (2006). Training for vigilance: the effect of knowledge of results format and dispositional optimism and pessimism on performance and stress. British Journal of Psychology, 97(1), 115–135. doi:10.1348/000712605X62768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Anderson, M. C. M., & Therriault, D. (2003). Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 66–73. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97–136. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, G., Chapman, P., Bowden, K., & Crundall, D. (2002). Visual search while driving: skill and awareness during inspection of the scene. Transportation Research Part F, 5(2), 87–97. doi:10.1016/S1369-8478(02)00008-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verster, J. C., & Roth, T. (2012). Drivers can poorly predict their own driving impairment: a comparison between measurements of subjective and objective driving quality. Psychopharmacology, 219(3), 775–781. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2400-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ververs, P. M., & Wickens, C. D. (1998). Head-up displays: effects of clutter, display intensity and display location on pilot performance. The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 8(4), 377–403. doi:10.1207/s15327108ijap0804_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, C., Zhao, G., Lin, B., & Lee, J. (2013). Navigating a car in an unfamiliar country using an internet map: effects of street language formats, map orientation consistency, and gender on driver performance, workload and multitasking strategy. Behaviour & Information Technology, 32(5), 425–437. doi:10.1080/0144929X.2011.566941.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest in this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sean R. Green.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Green, S.R., Redford, J. Metasearch accuracy for letters and symbols: do our intuitions match empirical reality?. Metacognition Learning 11, 237–256 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-015-9143-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-015-9143-5

Keywords

Navigation