Skip to main content
Log in

If it is stored in my memory I will surely retrieve it: anatomy of a metacognitive belief

  • Published:
Metacognition and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Retrieval failures—moments when a memory will not come to mind—are a universal human experience. Yet many laypeople believe human memory is a reliable storage system in which a stored memory should be accessible. I predicted that people would see retrieval failures as aberrations and predict that fewer retrieval failures would happen in the future. After responding to a set of trivia questions, participants were asked whether they would do better, about the same, or worse if they were given a different, but equally difficult, set of questions to answer. The majority of participants said they would do about the same, but more participants said they would do better next time than said they would do worse, although these participants did not actually do better. This finding was especially pronounced when participants were given feedback, suggesting that hindsight bias—the feeling, which emerges when an answer is presented, that one knew it all along—contributed to participants’ belief that they had underperformed on the first set of questions. The finding that metacognitive judgments were influenced by beliefs stands out in a literature full of studies in which beliefs fail to influence judgments.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alicke, M. D., Klotz, M. L., Breitenbecher, D. L., & Yurak, T. J. (1995). Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(5), 804–825. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, A. S., & Bjork, R. A. (1996). Retrieval fluency as a metacognitive index. In L. M. Reder (Ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition (pp. 309–338). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, A. S., Bjork, R. A., & Schwartz, B. L. (1998). The mismeasure of memory: when retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(1), 55–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Besken, M., & Mulligan, N. W. (2013). Easily perceived, easily remembered? Perceptual interference produces a double dissociation between metamemory and memory performance. Memory & Cognition, 41(6), 897–903. doi:10.3758/s13421-013-0307-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 309–330). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1999). Assessing our own competence: Heuristics and illusions. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII: Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application (pp. 435–459). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self-regulated learning: beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 417–444. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cassady, J. C., & Johnson, R. E. (2002). Cognitive test anxiety and academic performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27(2), 270–295. doi:10.1006/ceps.2001.1094.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, J. R., & Windschitl, P. D. (2004). Biases in social comparative judgments: the role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 813–838. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finn, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2008). Judgments of learning are influenced by memory for past test. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(1), 19–34. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.03.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischhoff, B. (1975). Hindsight is not equal to foresight: the effects of outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 288–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1977). Knowing with certainty: the appropriateness of extreme confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3(4), 552–564. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.3.4.552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilbault, R. L., Bryant, F. B., Brockway, J. H., & Posavac, E. J. (2004). A meta-analysis of research on hindsight bias. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 26(2–3), 103–117. doi:10.1080/01973533.2004.9646399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guilmette, T. J., & Paglia, M. F. (2004). The public’s misconception about traumatic brain injury: a follow up survey. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology : The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 19(2), 183–189. doi:10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00025-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., & Kelley, C. M. (1987). Unconscious influences of memory for a prior event. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 314–336.

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Intuitive prediction: biases and corrective procedures. TIMS Studies in Management Science, 12, 313–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1993). How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. Psychological Review, 100(4), 609–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: a cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(4), 349–370. doi:10.1037//0096-3445.126.4.349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Lichtenstein, S., & Fischhoff, B. (1980). Reasons for confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 107–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Sheffer, L., & Ma’ayan, H. (2002). Comparing objective and subjective learning curves: judgments of learning exhibit increased underconfidence with practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(2), 147–162. doi:10.1037//0096-3445.131.2.147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Bjork, R. A., Sheffer, L., & Bar, S. K. (2004). Predicting one’s own forgetting: the role of experience-based and theory-based processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(4), 643–656. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Ma’ayan, H., Sheffer, L., & Bjork, R. A. (2006). Exploring a mnemonic debiasing account of the underconfidence-with-practice effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(3), 595–608. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2007). The promise and perils of self-regulated study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 219–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2009). A stability bias in human memory: Overestimating remembering and underestimating learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 449–468. doi:10.1037/a0017350.

  • Kornell, N., Rhodes, M. G., Castel, A. D., & Tauber, S. K. (2011). The ease of processing heuristic and the stability bias: dissociating memory, memory beliefs, and memory judgments. Psychological Science, 22(6), 787–794. doi:10.1177/0956797611407929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, J. (1999). Lake Wobegon be gone! The “below-average effect” and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 221–232. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10474208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1121–1134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, J., & Evans, M. (2004). If you don’t want to be late, enumerate: unpacking reduces the planning fallacy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(5), 586–598. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. F. (1996). Eyewitness testimony. Harvard University Press.

  • Magnussen, S., Andersson, J., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., Endestad, T., Goodman, G. S., et al. (2006). What people believe about memory. Memory, 14(5), 595–613. doi:10.1080/09658210600646716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J. (1998). Cognitive optimism: self-deception or memory-based processing heuristics? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(2), 100–110. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0202_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, M. L., Tauber, S. K., & Dunlosky, J. (2013). Contributions of beliefs and processing fluency to the effect of relatedness on judgments of learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(2), 378–384. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0343-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, M. L., Dunlosky, J., Tauber, S. K., & Rhodes, M. G. (2014). The font-size effect on judgments of learning: does it exemplify fluency effects or reflect people’s beliefs about memory? Journal of Memory and Language, 70, 1–12. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.09.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1980). Norms of 300 general-information questions: accuracy of recall, latency of recall, and feeling-of-knowing ratings. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 338–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. G., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Memory predictions are influenced by perceptual information: evidence for metacognitive illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(4), 615–625. doi:10.1037/a0013684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. G., & Castel, A. D. (2009). Metacognitive illusions for auditory information: effects on monitoring and control. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(3), 550–554. doi:10.3758/PBR.16.3.550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L., Benjamin, A. S., & Bjork, R. A. (1997). The inferential and experiential bases of metamemory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6(5), 132–137. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepperd, J. A., Klein, W. M. P., Waters, E. A., & Weinstein, N. D. (2013). Taking stock of unrealistic optimism. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(4), 395–411. doi:10.1177/1745691613485247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (2011). What people believe about how memory works: a representative survey of the U.S. population. PloS One, 6(8), e22757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., & Pearlstone, Z. (1966). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5(4), 381–391. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(66)80048-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1971). Belief in the law of small numbers. Psychological Bulletin, 76(2), 105–110. doi:10.1037/h0031322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wissman, K. T., Rawson, K. A., & Pyc, M. A. (2012). How and when do students use flashcards? Memory, 20(6), 568–579. doi:10.1080/09658211.2012.687052.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author note

This research was supported by a Scholar Award from the James S. McDonnell foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nate Kornell.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kornell, N. If it is stored in my memory I will surely retrieve it: anatomy of a metacognitive belief. Metacognition Learning 10, 279–292 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-014-9125-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-014-9125-z

Keywords

Navigation