Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Young children’s narrative skill: concurrent and predictive associations with emergent literacy and early word reading skills

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Narrative skill is included in emergent literacy frameworks and believed to be important for children’s early reading development. Yet, empirical evidence concerning associations with other emergent literacy skills and later word reading skills is limited. We comprehensively assessed the emergent literacy skills of 3- to 5.5-year old children (n = 243), along with their word identification and decoding skills 2 years later. Narrative skill was modestly associated with all measures of emergent literacy. Narrative skill predicted word reading skills in univariate models but not after accounting for other emergent literacy skills. Further analyses showed that associations between narrative and word reading skills were fully mediated by other emergent literacy skills. When considered in light of prior work indicating associations between narrative skill and reading comprehension, these indirect associations between narrative and early word reading suggest a second pathway by which narrative skill may support reading development.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Armand, F., Lefrançois, P., Baron, A., Gomez, M. C., & Nuckle, S. (2004). Improving reading and writing learning in underprivileged pluri-ethnic settings. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 437–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barra, G., & McCabe, A. (2013). Oral narrative skills of chilean preschool children. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 32, 367–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blankman, C., Teglasi, H., & Lawser, M. (2002). Thematic apperception, narrative schemas, and literacy. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 20, 268–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bond, T., & Fox, C. M. (2015). Applying the Rasch model: Fundamental measurement in the human sciences (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudreau, D. M. (2008). Narrative abilities: Advances in research and implications for clinical practice. Topics in Language Disorders, 28, 99–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boudreau, D. M., & Hedberg, N. L. (1999). A comparison of early literacy skills in children with specific language impairment and their typically developing peers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 249–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourg, T., Bauer, P., & van den Broek, P. (1997). Building the bridges: The development of event comprehension and representation. In P. van den Broek, P. Bauer, & T. Bourg (Eds.), Developmental spans in event comprehension and representation: Bridging fictional and actual events (pp. 385–407). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, R. P., Justice, L. M., Khan, K. S., Piasta, S. B., Skibbe, L. E., & Foster, T. D. (2018). Development of the Narrative Assessment Protocol-2 (NAP-2): A tool for examining young children’s narrative skills. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, D. H., Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Schatschneider, C. (2002). The contribution of oral language skills to the development of phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 399–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D. K. (2011). Teachers’ language practices and academic outcomes of preschool children. Science, 333, 964–967. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D. K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2010). Speaking out for language: Why language is central to reading development. Educational Researcher, 39, 305–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D. K., & McCabe, A. (2001). Bringing it all together: The multiple origins, skills, and environmental supports of early literacy. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 16, 186–202. https://doi.org/10.1111/0938-8982.00019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. O. (2001). Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebert, K. D., & Scott, C. M. (2014). Relationships between narrative language samples and norm-referenced test scores in language assessments of school-age children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 45, 337–350. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_LSHSS-14-0034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engel, S. (1995). The stories children tell: Making sense of the narratives of childhood. New York, NY: WH Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, W. P. (1991). The Rasch debate: Validity and revolution in educational measurement. In M. Wilson (Ed.), Objective measurement: Theory into practice (Vol. II, pp. 36–72). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner-Neblett, N., & Iruka, I. U. (2015). Oral narrative skills: Explaining the language-emergent literacy link by race/ethnicity and ses. Developmental Psychology, 51, 889–904.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillam, R. B., & Pearson, N. (2004). Test of narrative language. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glasgow, C., & Cowley, J. (1994). Renfrew Bus Story Test—North (American ed.). Centreville, DE: Centreville School.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, T. M., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. P. (2004). Oral discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First Language, 24, 123–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, Y. R., & Schroeder, V. M. (2012). What the Berenstain Bears can tell us about school readiness: Maternal story grammar style and preschool narrative recall. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 10, 176–195. https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718x11430072.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilmann, J., Miller, J. F., Nockerts, A., & Dunaway, C. (2010). Properties of the narrative scoring scheme using narrative retells in young school-age children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 154–166. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2009/08-0024).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hipfner-Boucher, K., Milburn, T., Weitzman, E., Greenberg, J., Pelletier, J., & Girolametto, L. (2014). Relationships between preschoolers’ oral language and phonological awareness. First Language, 34, 178–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, D. L., McGillivray, L., & Schmidek, M. (1997). Guide to narrative language: Procedures for assessment. Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. (2002). Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 337–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Justice, L. M., Bowles, R., Pence, K., & Gosse, C. (2010a). A scalable tool for assessing children's language abilities within a narrative context: The NAP (Narrative Assessment Protocol). Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 218–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Justice, L. M., Bowles, R. P., & Skibbe, L. E. (2006). Measuring preschool attainment of print-concept knowledge: A study of typical at-risk 3- to 5-year-old children using item response theory. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 37, 224–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Justice, L. M., McGinty, A., Piasta, S. B., Kaderavek, J. N., & Fan, X. (2010b). Print-focused read-alouds in preschool classrooms: Intervention effectiveness and moderators of child outcomes. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 504–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Justice, L. M., Pullen, P. C., & Pence, K. (2008). Influence of verbal and nonverbal references to print on preschoolers’ visual attention to print during storybook reading. Developmental Psychology, 44, 855–866. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. S. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: The independent contributions of oral language and decoding skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 765–778. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015956.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Language and Reading Research Consortium. (2015a). The dimensionality of language ability in young children. Child Development, 86, 1948–1965. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Language and Reading Research Consortium. (2015b). Learning to read: Should we keep things simple? Reading Research Quarterly, 50, 151–169. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linacre, J. M. (2015). Winsteps (Version 3.90) [computer software]. Beaverton, OR: winsteps.com.

  • Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, J. S., van den Broek, P., Kremer, K. E., Kendeou, P., White, M. J., & Lorch, E. P. (2008). The development of narrative comprehension and its relation to other early reading skills. Reading Psychology, 29, 327–365. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702710802165416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, J. M., & Stewart, J. P. (1990). Emergent literacy assessment for instructional use in kindergarten. In L. M. Morrow & J. K. Smith (Eds.), Assessment for instruction in early literacy (pp. 155–175). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough, A., Bird, S., Tourigny, E., Romaine, A., Graham, S., Ottmann, J., et al. (2008). Storytelling as a foundation to literacy development for aboriginal children: Culturally and developmentally appropriate practices. Canadian Psychology, 49, 148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merritt, D. D., & Liles, B. Z. (1989). Narrative analysis: Clinical applications of story generation and story retelling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 438–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. F., Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., Iglesias, A., Fabiano, L., & Francis, D. J. (2006). Oral language and reading in bilingual children. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 21, 30–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy. Washington, D. C.: National Institute for Literacy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuman, S. B. (1996). Children engaging in storybook reading: The influence of access to print resources, opportunity, and parental interaction. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 11, 495–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Child care and development: Results from the NICHD study of early child care and youth development. New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perline, R., Wright, B. D., & Wainer, H. (1979). The Rasch model as additive conjoint measurement. Applied Psychological Measurement, 3, 237–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piasta, S. B., Phillips, B. M., Williams, J. M., Bowles, R. P., & Anthony, J. L. (2016). Measuring young children’s alphabet knowledge: Development and validation of brief letter-sound knowledge assessments. The Elementary School Journal, 116, 523–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. https://doi.org/10.3758/brm.40.3.879.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasch, G. (1993). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests (extended edition). Chicago: University of Chicago Press (Original work published 1960).

  • Reese, E., Suggate, S., Long, J., & Schaughency, E. (2010). Children’s oral narrative and reading skills in the first 3 years of reading instruction. Reading and Writing, 23, 627–644. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9175-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, F. P. (2000). Narrative writing: Development and teaching with children with writing difficulties. Topics in Language Disorders, 20, 15–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Cooper, D. H. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of the connection between oral language and early reading. The Journal of Educational Research, 95, 259–272. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220670209596600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 1, pp. 97–110). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2001). Storybook reading and parent teaching: Links to language and literacy development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 92, 39–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 445–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2014). Continuity and change in the home literacy environment as predictors of growth in vocabulary and reading. Child Development, 85, 1552–1568. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., LeFevre, J.-A., Smith-Chant, B. L., & Colton, K. V. (2001). On refining theoretical models of emergent literacy the role of empirical evidence. Journal of School Psychology, 39, 439–460. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4405(01)00081-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & Lever, R. (2014). Young children’s narrative abilities: Links to syntax comprehension and reading. In S. Quinn & S. Robson (Eds.), International handbook of young children’s thinking and understanding (pp. 96–108). Oxford: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harvard Educational Review, 53, 165–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1991). The theoretical basis for relationships between language and literacy in development. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6, 5–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Tabors, P. O., Nicholson, P. A., & Kurland, B. F. (1995). SHELL: Oral language and early literacy skills in kindergarten and first-grade children. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 10, 37–48. https://doi.org/10.1080/02568549509594686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Speece, D., Roth, F., Cooper, D., & De la paz, S.(1999). The relevance of oral language skills to early literacy. A multivariate analysis. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 167–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934–947. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabors, P. O., Roach, K. A., & Snow, C. E. (2001). Home language and literacy environment: Final results. In D. K. Dickinson & P. O. Tabors (Eds.), Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school (pp. 111–138). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teale, W., & Sulzby, E. (1986). Emergent literacy: Writing and reading. Norwood, NJ: Albex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tortorelli, L., Bowles, R. P., & Skibbe, L. E. (in press). Easy as AcHGzrjq: The quick letter name knowledge assessment (Q-lnk). The Reading Teacher.

  • Treiman, R., Schmidt, J., Decker, K., Robins, S., Levine, S. C., & Demir, Ö. E. (2015). Parents’ talk about letters with their young children. Child Development, 86, 1406–1418. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census Bureau. (2015). Children characteristics: 2011-2015 American Community Survey 5-year estimates Retrieved March 23, 2017, from factfinder.census.gov/faces/tables ervices/jsf/pages/productview.xhtml?pid = ACS_15_5YR_S0901&prodType = table.

  • Verhoeven, L., van Leeuwe, J., & Vermeer, A. (2011). Vocabulary growth and reading development across the elementary school years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 8–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2011.536125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, R. L., Lewis, B. A., Freebairn, L. A., Avrich, A. A., Hansen, A. J., & Stein, C. M. (2011). Narrative ability of children with speech sound disorders and the prediction of later literacy skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 42, 561–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiig, E. H., Secord, W. A., & Semel, E. (2004). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals preschool (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001, 2007). Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement (3rd ed., normative update). Itasca, IL: Riverside.

  • Zevenbergen, A. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2003). Dialogic reading: A shared picture book reading intervention for preschoolers. In A. van Kleeck, S. A. Stahl, & E. B. Bauer (Eds.), On reading books to children: Parents and teachers (pp. 177–200). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, F. J., Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Christakis, D. A., Xu, D., Gray, S., et al. (2009). Teaching by listening: The importance of adult-child conversations to language development. Pediatrics, 124, 342–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The research reported here was supported by the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education, through Grant R305A110293 awarded to Michigan State University (Bowles, Skibbe) and The Ohio State University (Justice, Piasta). The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views of the Institute or the U.S. Department of Education. The authors gratefully acknowledge the research staff and participating children/families without whom this research would not be possible.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shayne B. Piasta.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 5 Description of reviewed studies by sample, narrative measure, and outcomes examined

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Piasta, S.B., Groom, L.J., Khan, K.S. et al. Young children’s narrative skill: concurrent and predictive associations with emergent literacy and early word reading skills. Read Writ 31, 1479–1498 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9844-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9844-7

Keywords

Navigation