Skip to main content
Log in

Tracking the mind during writing: immediacy, delayed, and anticipatory effects on pauses and writing rate

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examines the dynamics of cognitive processes during writing. Participants were 5th, 7th and 9th graders ranging in age from 10 to 15 years. They were shown a short silent video composed of clips illustrating conflictual situations between people in school, and were invited to produce a narrative text. Three chronometric measures of word n were analyzed using a Linear Mixed-Effects Model regression procedure: pause duration before word n, pause duration within word n, and writing rate of word n. The predictors were sublexical and lexical properties of word n, i.e., immediacy effects, word n − 1, i.e., delayed effects, and of word n + 1, i.e., anticipatory effects. The writing-rate and the intra-word-pause measures show both immediacy and anticipatory effects. However, the between-word-pause durations show only delayed effects, which has not been reported in previous studies. As far as we know, our study is the first investigation which reveals the occurrence of parallel and serial effects in written text production: preprocessing of word n + 1 occurs when word n is being written, and properties of word n − 1 still exert their influences while the pen has already moved to the next word.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alamargot, D., & Chanquoy, L. (2001). Through the models of writing. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Alamargot, D., Chesnet, D., Dansac, C., & Ros, C. (2006). Eye and Pen: A new device to study reading during writing. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 287–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Feldman, L. B., & Schreuder, R. (2006). Morphological influences on the recognition of monosyllabic monomorphemic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 290–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., & Swanson, H. L. (1994). Modifying Hayes and Flower’s model of skilled writing to explain beginning and developing writing. In E. Butterfield (Ed.), Children’s writing: Toward a process theory of the development of skilled writing (pp. 57–81). Greenwich, CT: J.A.I. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonin, P., Fayol, M., & Gombert, J. E. (1997). The role of phonological and orthographic codes in picture naming and writing. CPC/Current Psychology of Cognition, 16, 299–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonin, P., Peereman, R., & Fayol, M. (2001). Do phonological codes constraint the selection of orthographic codes in written picture naming? Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 688–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chanquoy, L., Foulin, J.-N., & Fayol, M. (1990). The temporal management of short text writing by children and adults. European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology, 10, 513–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chesnet, D., & Alamargot, D. (2005). Analyse en temps réel des activités oculaires graphomotrices du scripteur. Intérêt du dispositif Eye and pen. L’Année Psychologique, 105, 477–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delattre, M., Bonin, P., & Barry, C. (2006). Written spelling to dictation: Sound-to-spelling regularity affects both writing latencies and durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1330–1340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A. W. (1982). Spelling and writing (and reading and speaking). In A. W. Ellis (Ed.), Normality and pathology in cognitive functions (pp. 113–143). New-York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M. (1991). From sentence production to text production: Investigating fundamental processes. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 6, 101–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M. (1999). From on-line management problems to strategies in written composition. In M. Torrance & G. Jeffery (Eds.), The cognitive demands of writing: Processing capacity and working memory effects (pp. 13–23). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1980). The dynamic of composing: Making plans and juggling constraints. In L. W. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 31–50). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M., & Holmes, V. M. (1978). Planning units and syntax in sentence production. Cognition, 6, 35–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foulin, J.-N. (1998). To what extent does pause location predict pause duration in adults’ and children’s writing ? Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 17, 601–620.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. R., & Flower, L. S. (1980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In L. W. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 3–30). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, T., & Grabowski, J. (1995). Pre-terminal levels of process in oral and written language production. In U. M. Quasthoff (Ed.), Aspects of oral communication (pp. 67–87). Berlin, G: Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87, 329–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kandel, S., Alvarez, C., & Vallée, N. (2006). Syllables as processing units in handwriting production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 18–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kandel, S., Herault, L., Grosjacques, G., Lambert, E., & Fayol, M. (2009). Orthographic vs. phonologic syllables in handwriting production. Cognition, 110, 440–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, S. T., Boches, C. A., Trabert, M. L., & Logan, G. D. (1991). Automatizing alphabet arithmetic: II. Are there practice effects after automaticity is achieved? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 196–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kliegl, R. (2007). Toward a perceptual-span theory of distributed processing in reading: A reply to Rayner, Pollatsek, Drieghe, Slattery, & Reichle (2007). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 530–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kliegl, R., Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R. (2006). Tracking the mind during reading: The influence of past, present, and future words on fixation duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 12–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kolers, P. A. (1976). Buswell’s discoveries. In R. A. Monty & J. W. Senders (Eds.), Eye movements and psychological processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuperman, V., Dambacher, M., Nuthmann, A., & Kliegl, R. (2010). The effect of word position on eye-movements in sentence and paragraph reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1838–1857.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, E., Kandel, S., Fayol, M., & Esperet, E. (2007). The effect of the number of syllables when writing poly-syllabic words. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 859–883.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lété, B. (2003). Building the mental lexicon by exposure to print: A corpus-based analysis of French reading books. In P. Bonin (Ed.), Mental lexicon. “Some words to talk about words” (pp. 187–214). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lété, B., Peereman, R., & Fayol, M. (2008). Phoneme-to-grapheme consistency and word-frequency effects on spelling among first-to-fifth-grade French children: A regression-based study. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 952–977.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lété, B., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Colé, P. (2004). Manulex: A grade-level lexical database from French elementary-school readers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 156–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Klapp, S. T. (1991). Automatizing alphabet arithmetic: I Is extended practice necessary to produce automaticity? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 179–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuhashi, A. (1982). Explorations in the real-time production of written discourse. In M. Nystrand (Ed.), What writers know: The language, process, and structure of written discourse (pp. 269–290). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition. Educational Psychology Review, 8(3), 299–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D. (2000). Knowledge, processing, and working memory: Implications for a theory of writing. Educational Psychologist, 35, 13–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peereman, R., & Content, A. (1997). Orthographic and phonological neighborhoods in naming: Not all neighbors are equally influential in orthographic space. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 382–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peereman, R., Lété, B., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2007). Manulex-infra: Distributional characteristics of grapheme-phoneme mappings, infra-lexical and lexical units in child-directed written material. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 593–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roux, S., & Bonin, P. (2009). Neighborhood effects in spelling in adults. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 369–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schilperoord, J. (1996). It’s about time: Temporal aspects of cognitive Processes in text production. Amsterdam/Atlanta: Rodopi.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schilperoord, J. (2002). On the cognitive status of pauses in discourse production. In T. Olive & C. M. Levy (Eds.), Contemporary tools and techniques for studying writing (pp. 61–90). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Plaats, R. E., & Van Galen, G. E. (1990). Effects of spatial and motor demands in handwriting. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22(3), 361–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Galen, G. P. (1991). Handwriting: Issues for a psychomotor theory. Human Movement Science, 10, 165–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Hell, J. G., Verhoeven, L., & Van Beijsterveldt, L. M. (2008). Pause time patterns in writing narrative and expository texts by children and adults. Discourse Processes, 45, 406–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L., & Van Hell, J. G. (2008). From knowledge representation to writing text: A developmental perspective. Discourse Processes, 45, 387–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Severine Maggio.

Additional information

This work was supported by a grant from the Agence Nationale pour la Recherche (ANR05 BLAN 0313) Harriet Jisa, PI.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Maggio, S., Lété, B., Chenu, F. et al. Tracking the mind during writing: immediacy, delayed, and anticipatory effects on pauses and writing rate. Read Writ 25, 2131–2151 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9348-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9348-1

Keywords

Navigation