Skip to main content
Log in

When neutral is not neutral: Neurophysiological evidence for reduced discrimination between aversive and non-aversive information in generalized anxiety disorder

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) is characterized by a range of cognitive and affective disruptions, such as pathological worry. There is debate, however, about whether such disruptions are specifically linked to heightened responses to aversive stimuli, or due to overgeneralized threat monitoring leading to deficits in the ability to discriminate between aversive and non-aversive affective information. The present study capitalized on the temporal and functional specificity of scalp-recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) to examine this question by exploring two targeted neurocognitive responses in a group of adults diagnosed with GAD: (1) visual processing of angry (aversive) versus neutral (non-aversive) faces; and (2) response monitoring of incorrect (aversive) versus correct (non-aversive) responses. Electroencephalography was recorded while 15 adults with GAD and 15 age-matched controls viewed angry and neutral faces prior to individual trials of a flanker task. ERPs to faces were the P1, reflecting attention allocation, the early posterior negativity (EPN), reflecting early affective discrimination, and the N170, reflecting face-sensitive visual discrimination. The error-related negativity (ERN) and positivity (Pe) were generated to incorrect and correct responses. Results showed reduced discrimination between aversive and non-aversive faces and responses in the GAD relative to the control group during visual discrimination (N170) and later-emerging error monitoring (Pe). These effects were driven by exaggerated processing of non-aversive faces and responses, suggesting over-generalized threat monitoring. Implications for cognitive-affective models of GAD are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The SCID is a semi-structured interview that assesses the presence and severity of DSM-IV defined mental disorders.

  2. All interviewers were trained over a 6-month period in diagnostic interviewing with the SCID. Reliability was determined via the clinician severity rating (CSR) from the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV (ADIS-IV; DiNardo et al. 1994). The CSR is a 0–8 rating of the severity of symptoms and impairment associated with each diagnosis that was evaluated between the interviewer and an expert diagnostician (Ph.D.), who provided ratings following a dispositional case presentation. Reliability was examined in 60% (9/15) of participants. There was 100% agreement (k = 1.00, p < .001) such that CSR ratings were at or above the clinical level of severity (4). Clinical participants had a primary diagnosis of GAD according to the SCID and the highest CSR rating when comorbidity was present.

  3. When possible, analyses were conducted with these additional participants and results remained the same.

  4. Actor numbers used: 5,7,8,10,11,12,13,14,20,21,23,33,38,39,41,43.

  5. Electrodes and time windows were selected based on topographical maps generated from the combined sample (GAD and Control).

  6. Multiple comparisons were corrected for using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995) that ranks p values and accounts for the number of comparisons made. Corrections were applied separately for each family of ERPs (stimulus-locked and response-locked) with anxiety questionnaires. The raw p value is reported and was significant using a false discovery rate criterion of 0.25.

  7. ERN was also examined as ERN-CRN difference scores (∆ERN) and at peak amplitudes. For peak amplitudes the same main effects of Correctness and Condition were replicated.

References

  • Aarts, K., De Houwer, J., & Pourtois, G. (2012). Evidence for the automatic evaluation of self-generated actions. Cognition, 124(2), 117–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aarts, K., De Houwer, J., & Pourtois, G. (2013). Erroneous and correct actions have a different affective valence: Evidence from ERPs. Emotion, 13(5), 960.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aldao, A., & Mennin, D. S. (2012). Paradoxical cardiovascular effects of implementing adaptive emotion regulation strategies in generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50(2), 122–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Amir, N., Beard, C., Burns, M., & Bomyea, J. (2009). Attention modification program in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ballenger, J. C., Davidson, J. R., Lecrubier, Y., Nutt, D. J., Borkovec, T. D., Rickels, K., … Wittchen, H.-U. (2001). Consensus statement on generalized anxiety disorder from the International Consensus Group on Depression and Anxiety. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, (62(Suppl 11), 53–58.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., & McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(6), 551–565.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cha, J., Greenberg, T., Carlson, J. M., DeDora, D. J., Hajcak, G., & Mujica-Parodi, L. R. (2014). Circuit-wide structural and functional measures predict ventromedial prefrontal cortex fear generalization: Implications for generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(11), 4043–4053. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3372-13.2014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis-Tiwary, T. A., Egan, L. J., Babkirk, S., & Denefrio, S. (2016). For whom the bell tolls: Neurocognitive individual differences in the acute stress-reduction effects of an attention bias modification game for anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 77, 105–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.12.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DiNardo, P. A., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1994). Anxiety disorders interview schedule for DSM–IV. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M. (2000). Event-related brain potentials distinguish processing stages involved in face perception and recognition. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111(4), 694–705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eldar, S., Yankelevitch, R., Lamy, D., & Bar-Haim, Y. (2010). Enhanced neural reactivity and selective attention to threat in anxiety. Biological Psychology, 85(2), 252–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Endrass, T., Reuter, B., & Kathmann, N. (2007). ERP correlates of conscious error recognition: Aware and unaware errors in an antisaccade task. European Journal of Neuroscience, 26(6), 1714–1720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Endrass, T., Schuermann, B., Kaufmann, C., Spielberg, R., Kniesche, R., & Kathmann, N. (2010). Performance monitoring and error significance in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychology, 84(2), 257–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Etkin, A., Prater, K. E., Hoeft, F., Menon, V., & Schatzberg, A. F. (2010). Failure of anterior cingulate activation and connectivity with the amygdala during implicit regulation of emotional processing in generalized anxiety disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(5), 545–554.

    Google Scholar 

  • Etkin, A., Prater, K. E., Schatzberg, A. F., Menon, V., & Greicius, M. D. (2009). Disrupted amygdalar subregion functional connectivity and evidence of a compensatory network in generalized anxiety disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(12), 1361–1372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., & Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 78(6), 447–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., Christ, S., & Hohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: A tutorial. Biological Psychology, 51(2–3), 87–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fava, M., Rankin, M. A., Wright, E. C., Alpert, J. E., Nierenberg, A. A., Pava, J., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (2000). Anxiety disorders in major depression. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 41(2), 97–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-440X(00)90140-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (2002). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I disorders, research version, non-patient edition (SCID-I/NP). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frenkel, T. I., & Bar-Haim, Y. (2011). Neural activation during the processing of ambiguous fearful facial expressions: An ERP study in anxious and nonanxious individuals. ResearchGate, 88(2–3), 188–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.08.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4(6), 385–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gehring, W. J., Himle, J., & Nisenson, L. G. (2000). Action-monitoring dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological Science, 11(1), 1–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55(4), 468–484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak, G., McDonald, N., & Simons, R. F. (2003). Anxiety and error-related brain activity. Biological Psychology, 64(1), 77–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hillyard, S. A., & Anllo-Vento, L. (1998). Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3), 781–787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, A., Nielsen, M. K., & Green, S. (2008). Effects of anxiety on the processing of fearful and happy faces: An event-related potential study. Biological Psychology, 77(2), 159–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.10.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd, C. B., Dien, J., & Coles, M. G. (1998). Error-related scalp potentials elicited by hand and foot movements: Evidence for an output-independent error-processing system in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 242(2), 65–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd, C. B., Nieuwenhuis, S., Yeung, N., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). Errors in reward prediction are reflected in the event-related brain potential. Neuroreport, 14(18), 2481–2484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Itier, R. J., & Taylor, M. J. (2004). N170 or N1? Spatiotemporal differences between object and face processing using ERPs. Cerebral Cortex, 14(2), 132–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, F., Nelson, B. D., & Proudfit, G. H. (2015). In an uncertain world, errors are more aversive: Evidence from the error-related negativity. Emotion, 15(1), 12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593–602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, E., Kang, J. I., Park, I. H., Kim, J.-J., & An, S. K. (2008). Is a neutral face really evaluated as being emotionally neutral? Psychiatry Research, 157(1), 77–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leuthold, H., & Sommer, W. (1999). ERP correlates of error processing in spatial SR compatibility tasks. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110(2), 342–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Li, X., Li, X., & Luo, Y.-J. (2005). Anxiety and attentional bias for threat: an event-related potential study. Neuroreport, 16(13), 1501–1505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lissek, S., Kaczkurkin, A. N., Rabin, S., Geraci, M., Pine, D. S., & Grillon, C. (2014). Generalized anxiety disorder is associated with overgeneralization of classically conditioned fear. Biological Psychiatry, 75(11), 909–915.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S. J., & Kappenman, E. K. (Eds.). (2011). The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacNamara, A., & Hajcak, G. (2010). Distinct electrocortical and behavioral evidence for increased attention to threat in generalized anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 27(3), 234–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacNamara, A., Kotov, R., & Hajcak, G. (2016). Diagnostic and symptom-based predictors of emotional processing in generalized anxiety disorder and major depressive disorder: An event-related potential study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40(3), 275–289. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9717-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, U., Brandeis, D., & McCandliss, B. D. (2005). Fast, visual specialization for reading in English revealed by the topography of the N170 ERP response. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 1(1), 13. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-1-13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mennin, D. S., & Fresco, D. M. (2014). Emotion regulation therapy. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed., pp. 469–490). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., & Fresco, D. M. (2002). Applying an emotion regulation framework to integrative approaches to generalized anxiety disorder. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 9(1), 85–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., & Fresco, D. M. (2005). Preliminary evidence for an emotion dysregulation model of generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(10), 1281–1310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(6), 487–495.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. (1991). The flanker compatibility effect as a function of visual angle, attentional focus, visual transients, and perceptual load: A search for boundary conditions. Perception & Psychophysics, 49(3), 270–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morel, S., George, N., Foucher, A., Chammat, M., & Dubal, S. (2014). ERP evidence for an early emotional bias towards happy faces in trait anxiety. Biological Psychology, 99, 183–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, M. G., Zuellig, A. R., Kachin, K. E., Constantino, M. J., Przeworski, A., Erickson, T., & Cashman-McGrath, L. (2002). Preliminary reliability and validity of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire-IV: A revised self-report diagnostic measure of generalized anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy, 33(2), 215–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Blom, J., Band, G. P., & Kok, A. (2001). Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: Evidence from an antisaccade task. Psychophysiology, 38(5), 752–760.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nitschke, J. B., Sarinopoulos, I., Oathes, D. J., Johnstone, T., Whalen, P. J., Davidson, R. J., & Kalin, N. H. (2009). Anticipatory activation in the amygdala and anterior cingulate in generalized anxiety disorder and prediction of treatment response. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(3), 302–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, R. G., Dockree, P. M., Bellgrove, M. A., Kelly, S. P., Hester, R., Garavan, H., … Foxe, J. J. (2007). The role of cingulate cortex in the detection of errors with and without awareness: A high-density electrical mapping study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25(8), 2571–2579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Toole, L. J., DeCicco, J. M., Berthod, S., & Dennis, T. A. (2013). The N170 to angry faces predicts anxiety in typically developing children over a two-year period. Developmental Neuropsychology, 38(5), 352–363. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2013.802321.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Olofsson, J. K., Nordin, S., Sequeira, H., & Polich, J. (2008). Affective picture processing: An integrative review of ERP findings. Biological Psychology, 77(3), 247–265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Overbeek, T. J., Nieuwenhuis, S., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dissociable components of error processing: on the functional significance of the Pe vis-à-vis the ERN/Ne. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19(4), 319–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulesu, E., Sambugaro, E., Torti, T., Danelli, L., Ferri, F., Scialfa, G., … Sassaroli, S. (2010). Neural correlates of worry in generalized anxiety disorder and in normal controls: A functional MRI study. Psychological Medicine, 40(1), 117–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, M., Williams, L., Young, A., Andrew, C., Bullmore, E., Brammer, M., … Gray, J. (2000). Differential neural responses to overt and covert presentations of facial expressions of fear and disgust. NeuroImage, 11(5), S232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pizzagalli, D. A., Peccoralo, L. A., Davidson, R. J., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). Resting anterior cingulate activity and abnormal responses to errors in subjects with elevated depressive symptoms: A 128-channel EEG study. Human Brain Mapping, 27(3), 185–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rousselet, G. A., Husk, J. S., Bennett, P. J., & Sekuler, A. B. (2008). Time course and robustness of ERP object and face differences. Journal of Vision, 8(12), 3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salters-Pedneault, K., Roemer, L., Tull, M. T., Rucker, L., & Mennin, D. S. (2006). Evidence of broad deficits in emotion regulation associated with chronic worry and generalized anxiety disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30(4), 469–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Santesso, D. L., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2009). The error-related negativity is related to risk taking and empathy in young men. Psychophysiology, 46(1), 143–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schupp, H. T., Flaisch, T., Stockburger, J., & Junghöfer, M. (2006a). Emotion and attention: Event-related brain potential studies. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 31–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schupp, H. T., Stockburger, J., Codispoti, M., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2006b). Stimulus novelty and emotion perception: The near absence of habituation in the visual cortex. Neuroreport, 17(4), 365–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, L. S., Tanaka, J. W., Sheinberg, D. L., & Curran, T. (2006). A reevaluation of the electrophysiological correlates of expert object processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(9), 1453–1465. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Segalowitz, S. J., Santesso, D. L., Murphy, T. I., Homan, D., Chantziantoniou, D. K., & Khan, S. (2010). Retest reliability of medial frontal negativities during performance monitoring. Psychophysiology, 47(2), 260–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E., Weinberg, A., Moran, T., & Hajcak, G. (2013). Electrocortical responses to NIMSTIM facial expressions of emotion. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 88(1), 17–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T., Larsen, J. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). May I have your attention, please: Electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 171–183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., & Gorsuch, R. L. (1983). State-trait anxiety inventory for adults: Manual and sample: Manual, instrument and scoring Guide. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stemmer, B., Segalowitz, S. J., Witzke, W., & Schönle, P. W. (2004). Error detection in patients with lesions to the medial prefrontal cortex: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 42(1), 118–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, J. W., & Curran, T. (2001). A neural basis for expert object recognition. Psychological Science, 12(1), 43–47. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., … Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168(3), 242–249.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., Luterek, J. A., Mennin, D. S., & Fresco, D. M. (2005). Emotion dysregulation in generalized anxiety disorder: A comparison with social anxiety disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 89–106. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-005-1651-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiology & Behavior, 77(4), 477–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vuilleumier, P., & Pourtois, G. (2007). Distributed and interactive brain mechanisms during emotion face perception: Evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 174–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinberg, A., & Hajcak, G. (2011). Electrocortical evidence for vigilance-avoidance in Generalized Anxiety Disorder. Psychophysiology, 48(6), 842–851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinberg, A., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Increased error-related brain activity distinguishes generalized anxiety disorder with and without comorbid major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(4), 885.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Weinberg, A., Kotov, R., & Proudfit, G. H. (2015). Neural indicators of error processing in generalized anxiety disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(1), 172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinberg, A., Olvet, D. M., & Hajcak, G. (2010). Increased error-related brain activity in generalized anxiety disorder. Biological Psychology, 85(3), 472–480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wieser, M. J., & Brosch, T. (2012). Faces in context: A review and systematization of contextual influences on affective face processing. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00471.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Xiao, Z., Wang, J., Zhang, M., Li, H., Tang, Y., Wang, Y., … Fromson, J. A. (2011). Error-related negativity abnormalities in generalized anxiety disorder and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 35(1), 265–272.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S., & Johnston, J. C. (1990). On the locus of visual selection: Evidence from focused attention tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(1), 135.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 111(4), 931.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, F., Zhu, C., Zhang, L., Chen, X., Li, D., Zhang, L., … Wang, K. (2015). The neural substrates of response inhibition to negative information across explicit and implicit tasks in GAD patients: Electrophysiological evidence from an ERP study. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 275–289.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was made possible by grant R56MH111700 and SC1MH104907 awarded to TDT from the National Institute of Mental Health and the National Institute of General Medical Sciences, respectively, and by grant TR000457 of the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tracy A. Dennis-Tiwary.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Denefrio, S., Myruski, S., Mennin, D. et al. When neutral is not neutral: Neurophysiological evidence for reduced discrimination between aversive and non-aversive information in generalized anxiety disorder. Motiv Emot 43, 325–338 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-018-9732-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-018-9732-0

Keywords

Navigation