Introduction

There is a growing body of research investigating the well-being of older adults, while living with chronic illnesses. This is an important area of research given the number of people living with chronic illnesses, which presents a different set of challenges than experiencing episodes of acute illness (Royer, 1998). Chronic illness is pervasive in American society and may well be the source of stressors, impacting the subjective physical well-being of older adults. More than 125 million Americans were estimated to have chronic diseases in the year 2000 and by 2020 that figure is expected to rise to 157 million (The Robert Wood Johnson Foundation, 2002). Furthermore, chronic diseases account for about 70% of deaths in America and 75% of the costs of health care each year (Marks, 2003). These figures are expected to be compounded over the next 30 years, as the aging of the population over 65 is expected to double. For these reasons, studying the effects of stress associated with chronic illness on subjective physical well-being is timely.

Although there has been a great deal of research in the stress literature, there has not been enough empirical research on the relationships of religious or spiritual factors and healthy lifestyle behaviors to the well-being of older adults, while living with chronic illness (Sulmasy, 2002). Some social scientists believe that the investigation of the effects of religious and spiritual resources in addition to healthy lifestyle behaviors may be important (Harris, 1999; Oppenheimer, 1999). Based on a convergence of scientific interest in social science and medical research, the preponderant view of the literature has been that older adults tend to take more interest in spiritual pursuits than younger people (Koenig, 1997; Moberg, 2001; Sheehan, 2001). Schultz-Hipp (2001) posits that this is true even while controlling for cohort effects.

Problem statement and hypotheses

The purpose of this study was to understand more about how older adults maintain their subjective physical well-being, while living with chronic illnesses. We also wanted to know whether maintaining their physical well-being is due to “coping” or “adaptation,” a rarely investigated concept. In order to study these questions we proposed three hypotheses: (1) Chronic illness related stressors negatively impact older adults’ experiences of physical well-being. (2) Spirituality and private religiosity contribute to higher physical well-being among older adults.

(3) Healthy lifestyle behaviors contribute to higher physical well-being of older adults.

These hypotheses were simultaneously tested in stress suppressor and distress deterrent models.

Theoretical background

Theories of stress and illness, along with supporting methodology, first evolved out of Selye’s (1956) biomedical research. Since social scientists began to study stress sociologically, there have been three major ways to conceptualize and measure stressors that threaten well-being: life events, daily hassles, and chronic stressors. This study investigated chronic stressors, which are those that may be brought on by long-term illness and may be prevalent stressors in late life. According to Pearlin (1980), they may be even more deleterious to the individual’s functional state than acute stressors because they are repetitive and last over a long period of time.

Generally, where chronic illness was experienced (Dekkers et al., 2001; Pakenham & Rinaldis, 2001), stress research has investigated buffers of stress in attempting to address factors that affect poor health outcomes. Furthermore, most of the stress-buffering models have studied how stress affected an outcome of physical disability instead of outcomes of well-being. Resources that may buffer the impairment process included medical care, therapeutic regimens, social support, physical and social environments, lifestyle behaviors, psychosocial and religious coping (Gall, 2000; Pakenham & Rinaldis, 2001; Stowell, Kiecolt-Glaser, & Glaser, 2001).

Guided by stress theory, both stress suppressor and distress deterrent models were tested in this study. As shown in Figure 1, the distress deterrent model is embedded in the stress suppressor model. Because of the complimentary relationship between these models in understanding stress effects and ameliorating effects of stressors on outcomes, this study tested both models simultaneously. The theoretical suppressor model (Wheaton, 1985) is an additive effects buffering model and is stress responsive (i.e., More stressors are related to a higher level of intervening resources which may lead to a higher level of well-being). The distress deterrent model is a stress counter-balancing model (Aneshensel, 1992; Krause & Van Tran, 1989; Wheaton, 1985) and allows for understanding non-stress responsive relationships (i.e., If stressors are not related to intervening resources, a higher level of well-being can still be experienced due to direct effects). In this study the suppressor model was used to understand the role of “coping” and the distress deterrent model was used to conceptualize the role of “adaptation” in ameliorating the effects of illness related stressors on physical well-being among older adults.

Figure 1
figure 1

Theoretical Model to Test Stress Suppressor and Embedded Distress Deterrent Relationships. Notes a. For Parsimony of Presentation, the Sociodemographics and Some Paths are not shown. b. Paths 2 and 5 Suppress the Relationship of path 7 in the Directions Shown. c. Paths 4 and 5, & 6 Respectively, Counter-Balance (Distress Deterrent) the Relationship of Path 7

Coping and adaptation in this study focused on religious and spiritual orientation and healthy life-style behaviors of chronically ill older adults to help lessen the effects of illness related stressors on their physical well-being. Coping is defined here as a subset of adaptation and involves effort commensurate with the level of stress (Compas, Connor, Osowiechi, and Welch, 1997). It is not an automatic action that takes place regardless of the presence of stressors. In addition, coping may also be a process that changes over time. On the other hand, adaptations are defined in this study as rituals and practices that are automatic, ingrained in the cultural fabric of the society in which individuals have developed over time and may be performed automatically or without effort as a part of everyday behavior (Lazarus & Folkman, 1991). Adaptations are not stress responsive and may take place regardless of the presence of stressors.

The suppressor model allowed for analyzing the ways in which the orientations serve as additive effects buffers. Complementary to this, the distress deterrent model showed how certain orientations and behaviors directly ameliorated the negative effects of stress on physical well-being. Using these two models, we were able to examine how each allowed for explaining whether religious and spiritual orientations and healthy behaviors, either through coping or adaptation, resulted in reducing illness related stressors.

Methods

Sample and data collection

The sample for this study includes 221 older adults, age 65 and older, who were part of an NIA funded study R01 AG 01658. That larger study was randomly selected from Medicare lists and known as the Cleveland Health Maintenance Study (CHMS). Participants lived in northeast Ohio, and at baseline in TIME1 1999, 365 older adults were interviewed face-to-face during the first wave of the study. During the two-year follow-up, 343 respondents were interviewed, reflecting a response rate at TIME 2 of 94%. However, only 221 of the participants were used in this study, representing those who responded to a TIME2 follow-up interview.

Attrition

Table 1 illustrates the characteristics of those respondents who were used in the follow-up interview and those who were lost to attrition due to death, refusal, and loss to follow-up. There were significant differences between the sample characteristics of those respondents who were used in the follow-up interview and those who were lost to attrition in the demographic categories of gender and race. More women and African Americans remained in the sample than were lost to attrition, but these two variables were not variables that were examined in this study. Additionally, there were no significant group differences between the retained sample and the attrition group in terms of age, education, public religiosity, private religiosity, illnesses, or healthy diet.

TABLE 1 Characteristics of Follow-up Study Respondents and Those Who Were Lost to Attrition

The sample characteristics

Characteristics of the sample are illustrated in Table 2. Additional descriptive statistics (not shown) revealed that 86% of the sample data were in the 75 or older age category, which consisted of 76% females and 24% males. Approximately 82% of the sample was White and 18% was Black. People in the lower socioeconomic statuses were notably represented in the sample with about one third having less than a high school education and 40% having a household income below $15,000 per year. In addition, a majority of the sample (93%) reported having been diagnosed with at least 1 chronic disease on the OARS Chronic Illness Inventory (George & Fillenbaum, 1985).

TABLE 2 Descriptive Statistics of the Sample Variables

Measures

Demographic and other control variables

Age and education were measured as continuous variables. Race was coded as White = 0 and Black = 1. Gender was coded as male = 0 and female = 1. Income was recoded with factors from 2.5 to 75, based on mean values of $5000 increments, and divided by 1000.

Subjective physical well-being

It was a 3 item composite created from CHMS survey items and reverse coded on 5-point Likert scales, with a reliability of Cronbach’s α = .78 for the sample: (1) In general, do you consider yourself to be a very healthy, healthy, fairly healthy, sick, or very sick person? from 1 (very sick) to 5 (very healthy); (2) Considering your health over the past year, would you say your health is excellent, good, fair, poor, or very poor? from 1 (very poor) to 5 (excellent); and (3) Compared to other people your age, would you say that your health is much better, better, about the same, worse, or much worse over the past year? from 1 (much worse) to 5 (much better). Composite scores ranged from 3 (low) to 15 (high), mean = 11.56, SD = 1.99.

Chronic illness

The OARS Chronic Illness Index (George & Fillenbaum, 1985) indicated if participants had any of the following 10 chronic illnesses over the past year: arthritis, asthma, emphysema, hypertension, heart trouble, diabetes, urinary tract problems, cancer, stroke, and orthopedic problems. Similar illness indices were supported in prior research as part of their research methodologies (Porell & Miltiades, 2001; Shaw & Krause, 2001). Scores ranged from 0 to 10 diseases, mean = 2.1, SD = 1.25.

Healthy lifestyle behaviors

The two measures used were physical activity and healthy diet. The term physical activity was used instead of exercise because many older adults engage in activities that may not be classified as exercise but may still be a part of an active lifestyle. Physical activity was measured as a continuous variable based on total hours per week that participants engaged in: walking as exercise, swimming, golfing, running, aerobics, stretching, weight lifting, dancing, bicycling or exercise machines, exercise for therapy or other exercises, household activities, gardening/lawn care, home repairs, and all other walking and activities. Scores ranged from 0 to 20 hours of activity, mean = 19.06, SD = 16.0. Participants’ adherence to healthy diets were assessed using responses from the CHMS questionnaire and measured on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much), mean = 3.97, SD = 1.11. Skewness (1.53) or kurtosis (3.01) on physical activity did not indicate violation of cutoffs (Kline, 1998), especially when you consider that the average age of the respondents was 80.

Spirituality

Spirituality was defined as experiencing transcendence through inner peace, harmony, or connectedness to others. Any one or all of these qualities could be present.

Four items, with an internal reliability of Cronbach’s α. = .79 for this sample, were used from The Daily Spiritual Experiences Scale (Fetzer, 1999). These four items were reverse coded and used to measure spirituality on 6-point Likert scales from 1 (never or almost never) to 6 (many times a day): (1) selfless caring for others, (2) connection to all of life, (3) deep inner peace or harmony, and (4) spiritually touched by the beauty of creation. A composite scale created from three Likert scales ranged from 6 (low) to 24 (high), mean = 17.03, SD = 4.04.

Religiosity

Religiosity was defined as the adherence to religious dogma or creed, the expression of moral beliefs, and/or the participation in organized or individual worship or sacred practices. Any one or all of these qualities may be present and the construct was measured in two dimensions as private and public religiosity. Private religiosity, with a reliability of Cronbach’s α = .95 for this sample, was a composite of 4 items. Three items from the Cope Inventory (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989) were each measured on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (never) to 5 (very often): (1) seek God’s help, (2) trust in God, (3) find comfort in religion. Also, one item from the CHMS 2001 questionnaire measured on a 5-point Likert scale, from 1 (never) to 5 (several times a week): (4) frequency of prayer. Composite scores of Likert scales ranged from 4 to 20, mean = 14.94, SD = 5.75.

Public religiosity, with a reliability of Cronbach’s α = .81 for this sample, was a composite of 3 religious participation items from the CHMS 2001 questionnaire. (1) frequency of attendance at religious services during the past year measured on a 5-point Likert scale, from 1 (never) to 5 (several times a week). (2) take part in other activities besides regular services, at your place of worship measured on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (never) to 5 (4 + times a week): (3) frequency of participation in other church or synagogue activities was reverse coded and measured on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (rarely or never) to 5 (several hours a day). Composite scores ranged from 3 to 15, mean = 6.52, SD = 2.86.

Preliminary data analysis

A number of tests were performed on the data. Limits for influential cases were computed and cutoffs for outliers were tested and confirmed acceptable. Diagnostics in the tests for assumptions of multiple regressions reflected non-linear patterns for some variables. This is not unusual among mostly chronically ill older adult populations (i.e., physical activity).

As a preliminary screening for possible multicollinearity in the model, a Pearson Correlation matrix was examined. Correlation results prompted running tolerance tests. Sample variable tolerances ranged from .511 to .921 (perfect tolerance = 1.00), illustrating moderate multicollinearity, given there are no perfect tolerance levels.

Results of face validity for the religiosity and spirituality constructs followed by Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) in SPSS were the basis for using a briefer version of The Daily Spiritual Experiences Scale (DSES) (Fetzer, 1999) consisting of only four items. These findings, illustrated in Table 3, support the work of Underwood and Teresi (2002), whose recent work testing content validity created a different brief DSES.

TABLE 3 Exploratory Factor Analysis of Religiosity and Spirituality

Moving forward in the analysis, path analysis was then used (Gogineni, Alsup, & Gillespie, 1995) to test the three hypotheses in the suppressor model. As a result of path analysis, the distress deterrent model emerged in the resulting significant paths, but the suppressor model did not result in significant paths.

Results

Predicting illness stressors, lifestyle, and religiosity or spirituality on physical well-being

The fit of the statistical suppressor model with the imbedded distress deterrent model was illustrated as a good fit to the data by the statistic F(11, 209) = 7.397, p < .001 and is appropriate for testing the hypotheses of this study. Therefore, the path coefficients, representing the suppressor and distress deterrent effects on the physical well-being of the older adults in the sample are illustrated in Table 4. After statistically testing the hypotheses, we determined whether religiosity, spirituality, or lifestyle behaviors were suppressors of chronic illness related stressors on physical well-being or whether they were instead distress deterrent factors which countered the effects of chronic illness related stressors on physical well-being.

TABLE 4 Path Analysis for Evaluating the Suppressor Model and the Distress Deterrent Statistical Model (N = 221)

Hypothesis 1: Chronic illness related stressors negatively impacted older adults’ experiences of physical well-being.

As predicted, chronic illness related stressors had a negative impact (β = −.326, p < .001) on physical well-being among older adults in the study. These results confirmed prior research showing the negative effects of chronic illnesses (Porell & Miltiades, 2001; Shaw & Krause, 2001). Therefore, hypothesis 1 could not be rejected.

Hypothesis 2: Spirituality and private religiosity contribute to higher physical well-being among older adults.

Hypothesis 3: Healthy lifestyle behaviors contribute to higher physical well-being of older adults.

Both of the healthy lifestyle behaviors, physical activities (β = .125, p < .05) and healthy diet (β = .222, p < .001), as well as spirituality (β = .164, p < .05) contributed to higher subjective physical well-being of the older adults in the sample. Figure 2 illustrates that only these direct effects, showing distress deterrent relationships (Krause & Van Tran, 1989; Wheaton, 1985), were significant effects of the intervening variables (lifestyle behaviors and spirituality). Based on theory, these relationships are adaptations, they are not responsive to stress and may be adaptations; hence they only had direct effects on physical well-being.

Figure 2
figure 2

Significant Paths Resulting from Testing the Theoretical Suppressor Model and Distress Deterrent Models. Bold paths and concepts represent only distress deterrent relationships. (a) Path coefficient of healthy diet is (β = .222, p ≤ .001). (b) Path coefficient of physical activity is (β = .125, p ≤ .05). (c) Path coefficient of spirituality (spiritual experiences) is (β = .164, p ≤ .05)

On the other hand, public religiosity was stress responsive (β = −.131, p < .05). Therefore, it was not a distress deterrent resource. However, the negative effect and insignificant relationship to subjective physical well-being indicates that it was not a stress suppressor either. In fact, not only was public religiosity not found to be a suppressor, but none of the intervening variables tested (public or private religiosity, spirituality, diet or physical activity) were found to exert suppressor effects in the model (See Figure 2). Contrast these findings with Figure 1 which illustrates the necessary positive paths (2 and 5) leading to and from the intervening variables in order to be considered as suppressor effects.

Revealing findings were that neither public nor private religiosity served any type of stress ameliorating functions; however spirituality (spiritual experiences) did act as a distress deterrent, counter-balancing the negative effects of stressors associated with chronic illness on physical well-being. Nevertheless, because of the stress counter-balancing effects of spirituality and healthy lifestyle behaviors (diet and physical activity), Hypotheses 2 and 3 could not be rejected.

Discussion

Three hypotheses were formed for this research. The first hypothesis stated that chronic illness related stressors negatively impact older adults’ experiences of physical well-being. Illness related stress effects on well-being may be partly due to a burden of stigma associated with ill health that older adults may carry, according to Baum, Jennings, Manuck, and Rabin (2000), which poses specific threats due to lasting changes in the physical self. This issue associated with self-image could compound the stress of having a chronic illness (Charmaz, 2000). Understanding stress effects of illness helps to know how some people make the most of life, experiencing well-being in the face of illness. It was useful in conceptualizing both stress and “coping” and stress and “adaptation” in both the suppressor model and the distress deterrent model, which was embedded in the stress suppressor model of Figure 1 and illustrated in Figure 2 (Krause & Van Tran, 1989).

The second hypothesis stated that spirituality and private religiosity contribute to higher physical well-being among older adults. As illustrated by the negative path 7 in Figure 1, chronically ill older adults with a mean age of 80 years were not expected to exhibit strong public religiosity, and results supported this. On the other hand, these data neither supported coping by private religiosity behaviors nor spirituality (e.g., daily spiritual experiences) as responses to stressors. Because of the absence of these positive significant relationships as illustrated in the theoretical model of Figure 2, a suppressor model was not supported as conceptualized in Figure 1. Instead of these suppressor relationships, distress deterrent relationships emerged in the data (See Figure 2); and although these results were conceptually different, the stress-ameliorating outcome was the same, thereby revealing that more spiritual experiences lead to higher subjective physical well-being. The conceptual difference was that unlike suppressor relationships, spirituality was not stress responsive (i.e., no significant positive relationship between chronic stressors and spirituality). Because of this, hypothesis two could not be rejected due to the stress counter-balancing effects illustrated in the distress deterrent relationships of Figure 2 for spirituality.

The third hypothesis stated that healthy lifestyle behaviors contribute to higher physical well-being of older adults. It was expected that healthy lifestyle behaviors would be lower, with more chronic illness related stressors; therefore, suppressor effects were not expected. Findings were that healthy lifestyle behaviors, diet and physical activity, were not affected by chronic illness related stressors but still contributed to higher subjective physical well-being. Therefore, hypothesis three could not be rejected because of the positive impact of the lifestyle behaviors on subjective physical well-being. As indicated by their spirituality, the healthy lifestyle behaviors of the older adults in this sample were not dependent upon chronic illnesses. Therefore, the positive impact of these behaviors on their subjective physical well-being may be seen as a means of adaptation (i.e., not stress responsiveness). In other words, the behaviors could have happened with or without illness stressors, and perhaps illness was not considered a stressor, but a by-product of old age. These positive effects counter-balanced the negative effects of chronic illness stressors. According to the theoretical framework used in this study, these results may illustrate adaptation. Adaptation is viewed as automatic and directly impacts the outcome (Lazarus & Folkman, 1991; Royer, 1998). This perspective is also applicable to the distress deterrent effects that were found for spirituality and healthy lifestyle behaviors on subjective physical well-being. On the other hand, coping intervenes between stressors and the outcome and has been described as a direct and effortful response to stress (Compas et al., 1997). These results appear to be consistent, at least in part, with Krause and Van Tran’s (1989) findings that religiosity was not stress responsive, but was counter-balancing (distress deterrent), in an older adult population.

The suppressor model is one way of illustrating the tenets of coping, while on the other hand, the distress deterrent model may better illustrate the characteristics of adaptation. This study revealed that the older adults had spiritual experiences and maintained their healthy lifestyle behaviors whether or not they were chronically ill. Therefore, adaptation as opposed to coping was more indicative of their spiritual orientation and lifestyle behaviors.

We believe that the results of this study contribute to the literature by shedding more light on a less explored concept about ways of modeling stress and coping versus adaptation. Studying stress in this broader context, which is often overlooked, distinguishes between the different concepts and better addresses a means of interpreting the findings. The findings of this study are important in revealing that not only stress responsive coping mechanisms contribute to well-being, but non-stress responsive effects can contribute to physical well-being through adaptation to life circumstances. In this study, it appeared that adaptation was taking place whereby the older adults practiced healthier lifestyle behaviors through diet and exercise even regardless of their chronic illnesses. Additionally, this study builds upon stress research with a positive outcome as the focus (well-being). Focusing on more positive outcomes in the face of chronic illness, Kahana and Kahana’s (1996, 2003) successful aging model emphasized the effects of lifestyles as proactive adaptations in ameliorating the effects of disability on subjective physical well-being. It also advances the seminal work of a prominent stress researcher, Antonovsky (1982), who wanted to know why some people experience greater well-being in the face of chronic illness than others.

We submit that the preponderant view of the literature is that older adults tend to take more interests in religious and spiritual pursuits than younger people (Moberg, 2001; Schultz-Hipp, 2001). However, this view did not explain why neither public nor private religiosity (trend or borderline significance) had significant effects on subjective physical well-being when tested in either the suppressor or the distress deterrent model. In order to address this question for public religiosity we considered the mean age (80 years) of respondents in the sample. With better than 86% of the sample over the age of 75 and an even higher percentage suffering from chronic illnesses, public religious participation, may not be a means of investigating religious involvement for the older adults in this study. Also, these data do indicate in descriptive statistics that most people relied on private religiosity, as evident by the mean level of public religiosity which was much lower compared to the mean level of private religiosity. This may be due to disability and lack of mobility associated with chronic illness. It may also partly explain the absence of significant effects of private religiosity on the outcome. Yet, since private religiosity is more intrinsic than public religiosity, not requiring public involvement, the results in these data do not readily explain the absence of either suppressor effects or distress deterrent effects on subjective physical well-being. Two considerations for these results are that the older adults in this study used negative adaptation in their private religiosity. It also appears that skew could have contributed to the lack of normal variability in the sample variable, and age of the sample could have affected the clustering on the scale and its low variability.

Study limitations

These findings must take into account the limitations of cross-sectional data, which may prevent accurate causal ordering at times. In addition, the religiosity measures may not generalize to elders who do not share Judeo-Christian beliefs. Despite these limitations, this research is an important start in investigating the impact of spirituality, religiosity, and lifestyle on subjective physical well-being among older adults, not only from the perspective of stress and coping but also in terms of stress and adaptation.

Furthermore, this study validates a short form of the Daily Spiritual Experiences Scale. Underwood and Teresi (2002) found non-generalizability was true for many items of the Daily Spiritual Experiences Scale (Fetzer, 1999). However, the factor analysis performed in this study helped to explain more about the construct validity of this measure.

Future studies need to include more emphasis on adaptation as well as coping. This will offer more avenues toward understanding the findings of stress research when including spirituality and religiosity in an aging population. Different dimensions of religiosity and spirituality may be more effectively explicated when compared in different theoretical frameworks (i.e., suppressor and distress deterrent models).