Skip to main content
Log in

Sensitivity to Phonological Universals: The Case of Stops and Fricatives

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Linguistic evidence suggests that syllables like bdam (with stop–stop clusters) are less preferred than bzam (with stop–fricative combinations). Here, we demonstrate that English speakers manifest similar preferences despite no direct experience with either structure. Experiment 1 elicited syllable count for auditory materials (e.g., does bzam have one syllable or two?); Experiment 2 examined the AX discrimination of auditory stimuli (e.g., is bzam \(=\) bezam?); whereas Experiment 3 repeated this task using printed materials. Results showed that syllables that are dispreferred across languages (e.g., bdam) were prone to misidentification relative to preferred syllables (e.g., bzam). The emergence of this pattern irrespective of stimulus modality—for auditory and printed materials—suggests that misidentification does not solely stem from a phonetic failure. Further, the effect remained significant after controlling for various statistical properties of the materials. These results suggest that speakers possess broad linguistic preferences that extend to syllables they have never encountered before.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The linguistic literature has proposed various sonority scales that differ in detail, ranging from five (Clements 1990) to seventeen levels (Parker 2008). For the sake of simplicity, we follow Selkirk (1984) and Parker (2002) in distinguishing the sonority levels of stops and fricatives, but in other respects, we use the rudimentary sonority scale proposed by Clements (1990). Our analyses disregard complex obstruent affricates (containing a stop and a fricative, e.g., the first sound in Joe) and treat sonority as an ordinal scale.

    Table 1 Sonority scale of speech sounds
  2. The restrictions on onset structure can acquire multiple forms—some directly appeal to sonority, whereas others do not (Smolensky 2006). We remain agnostic as to the exact representation of sonority restrictions in the language system—whether sonority is represented as a scalar phonological feature (c.f., Clements 1990) or whether it results from other constraints on feature conjunction (Smolensky 2006). Our question here is whether sonority can be used descriptively, to capture the well-formedness of the onset.

  3. Note that such counterexamples would incorrectly suggest that fricatives are less sonorous than stops, as segments allowed in the first onset position are typically more sonorous than their C2 counterparts.

  4. In the analysis of response time, the exclusion of trials faster than 200 ms and slower than 2.5 SD of mean response time yielded missing cells. By applying list-wise deletion, 4 participants with missing data were excluded from the subject analyses (N \(=\) 11) and 2 quartets with missing data were excluded from the item analyses (N \(=\) 10).

  5. All accuracy results were supported by a mixed-effect logit model, with obstruent type and well-formedness as fixed effects (both sum coded) and subject and quartet as a random effects (R Development Core Team 2011). The results confirmed the effect of sonority distance (\(\upbeta = -1.13, \hbox {SE} = .09, \hbox {Z} = -12.251, p<2^{\mathrm{e}}-16\)) and obstruent type (\(\upbeta = -.36, \hbox {SE} = 0.1, \hbox {Z} = -3.47, p<.0005\)) and no interaction (\(\upbeta = -.008, \hbox {SE} = .09, \hbox {Z} = -.09, p<.93\)). Similar models for disyllabic trials revealed no significant effects or interaction (all \(\upbeta = .22, p = .24\)).

  6. In stop-initial items, we defined the beginning of the vowel as the zero-crossing before the change in waveform amplitude and formant structure associated with the vowel, thus excluding stop closure and release. In fricative-initial items, we excluded fricative turbulence preceding the vowel. The end of the vowel was taken to be the zero-crossing before the stop closure and release (if the vowel was followed by a stop) and fricative turbulence (if it was followed by a fricative).

  7. The mixed effect logit models on response accuracy did not support the trends observed in ANOVAs (all \({it\ \upbeta } = .08\), p \(=\) .69). In the response time measure, no effects or interactions were significant (all \({ \upbeta } = .11.5\), p \(=\) .25).

  8. All accuracy results were supported by mixed-effect logit models (besides the sum coding of two-way contrasts syllable and obstruent type, we used forward difference coding for the three-way contrast sonority distance. Subject and sextet were included as random effects). The 2 syllable \(\times \) 2 obstruent type \(\times \) 3 sonority distance model confirmed yielded a marginal interaction of syllable and sonority distance \((\upbeta = 0.21786,\hbox {SE}=0.13129,\hbox {Z}=1.659,p<.097)\).

  9. The mixed-effects 2 obstruent type \(\times \) 3 sonority distance model yielded a marginally significant effect in the identical trials (\(\upbeta = 0.3957\), SE \(=\) 0.2081, Z \(=\) 1.902, \(p<.0572\)), thus confirming the effect we found in the corresponding ANOVA.

References

  • Barlow, J. (2005). Sonority effects in the production of consonant clusters by Spanish-speaking children. In Paper presented at the selected proceedings of the 6th conference on the acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as first and second languages.

  • Barlow, J. (2001). A preliminary typology of initial clusters in acquisition. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 15(1/2), 9–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., & Perfetti, C. A. (1995). A rose is a REEZ: The two-cycles model of phonology assembly in reading English. Psychological Review, 102(1), 146–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., Steriade, D., Lennertz, T., & Vaknin, V. (2007). What we know about what we have never heard: Evidence from perceptual illusions. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Cognition, 104(3), 591–630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., Lennertz, T., Jun, J., Moreno, M. A., & Smolensky, P. (2008). Language universals in human brains. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(14), 5321–5325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., Lennertz, T., Smolensky, P., & Vaknin-Nusbaum, V. (2009). Listeners’ knowledge of phonological universals: Evidence from nasal clusters. Phonology, 26(01), 75–108.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., & Lennertz, T. (2010). Universal constraints on the sound structure of language: Phonological or acoustic? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(1), 212–223.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., Balaban, E., Lennertz, T., & Vaknin-Nusbaum, V. (2010). Phonological universals constrain the processing of nonspeech stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3), 418–435. doi:10.1037/a0020094.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., Balaban, E., & Vaknin-Nusbaum, V. (2011a). How linguistic chickens help spot spoken-eggs: Phonological constraints on speech identification. Frontier in Language Sciences, 2, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00182.

  • Berent, I., Harder, K., & Lennertz, T. (2011b). Phonological universals in early childhood: Evidence from sonority restrictions. Language Acquisition, 18(4), 281–293.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., Lennertz, T., & Balaban, E. (2011c). Language universals and misidentification: A two way street. Language and Speech, 55, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blevins, J. (2006). A theoretical synopsis of Evolutionary Phonology. Theoretical Linguistics, 32(2), 117–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boersma, P., Weenink, D. (2003). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (version 5.2.32). Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/

  • Broselow, E., & Finer, D. (1991). Parameter setting in second language phonology and syntax. Second Language Research, 7, 35–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J., & McClelland, J. L. (2005). Alternatives to the combinatorial paradigm of linguistic theory based on domain general principles of human cognition. The Linguistic Review, 22, 381–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrd, D. (1992). Perception of assimilation in consonant clusters: A gestural model. Phonetica, 49, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christman, S. H. (1992). Uncovering phonological regularity in neologisms: Contributions of sonority theory. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 6, 219–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, G. N. (1990). The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification. In J. Kingston & M. Beckman (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology I: Between the grammar and physics of speech (pp. 282–333). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, L. (2010). Phonetic bases of similarities in cross-language production: Evidence from English and Catalan. Journal of Phonetics, 38(2), 272–288. doi:10.1016/j.wocn.2010.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, L. (2011a). Phonetic and phonological factors in the second language production of phonemes and phonotactics. Language and Linguistics Compass, 5(3), 126–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, L. (2011b). Phonetic, phonemic, and phonological factors in cross-language discrimination of phonotactic contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(1), 270–282.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, L. (2012). Sources of illusion in consonant cluster perception. Journal of Phonetics, 40(2), 234–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dell, F., & Elmedlaoui, M. (1985). Syllabic consonants and syllabification in Imdlawn Tashlhiyt Berber. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics, 7, 105–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dupoux, E., Kakehi, K., Hirose, Y., Pallier, C., & Mehler, J. (1999). Epenthetic vowels in Japanese: A perceptual illusion? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(6), 1568–1578.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dupoux, E., Parlato, E., Frota, S., Hirose, Y., & Peperkamp, S. (2011). Where do illusory vowels come from? Journal of Memory and Language, 63(3), 199–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckman, F. R., & Iverson, G. K. (1993). Sonority and markedness among onset clusters in the interlanguage of ESL learners. Second Language Research, 9, 234–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleischhacker, H. A. (2005). Similarity in phonology: Evidence from reduplication and loan adaptation. PhD. Los Angeles: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gierut, J. A. (1999). Syllable onsets: Clusters and adjuncts in acquisition. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 42(3), 708–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gnanadesikan, A. (2004). Markedness and faithfulness constraints in child phonology. In R. Kager, J. Pater, & W. Zonneveld (Eds.), Fixing priorities: Constraints in phonological acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gouskova, M. (2001). Falling sonority onsets, loanwords, and syllable contact. CLS 37: The main session. In Papers from the 37th meeting of the Chicago linguistic society (pp. 175–185).

  • Greenberg, J. H., & Jenkins, J. J. (1964). Studies in the psychological correlates of the sound system of American English. Word, 20, 157–177.

  • Greenberg, J. H. (1978). Some generalizations concerning initial and final consonant clusters. Universals in Human Language, 2, 243–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, B., & Wilson, C. (2008). A maximum entropy model of phonotactics and phonotactic learning. Linguistic Inquiry, 39, 379–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klippenstein, R. (2008). Word-initial consonant clusters in Albanian. Ohio State University Working Papers in Linguistics, 59, 10–32.

  • Ladefoged, P. (2001). A course in phonetics. Boston: Heinle & Heinle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lennertz, T., & Berent, I. (2013). People’s knowledge of phonological universals: Evidence from fricatives and stops. manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Massaro, D. W., & Cohen, M. M. (1983). Phonological constraints in speech perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 34, 338–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J. J. (1986). OCP effects: Gemination and antigemination. Linguistic Inquiry, 17, 207–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medler, D. A., & Binder, J. R. (2005). MCWord: An on-line orthographic database of the English language.

  • Moreton, E. (2002). Structural constraints in the perception of English stop-sonorant clusters. Cognition, 84(1), 55–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nusbaum, H. C., Pisoni, D. B., & Davis, C. K. (1984). Sizing up the Hoosier mental lexicon: Measuring the familiarity of 20,000 words. Research on Speech Perception Progress Report, 10, 357–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohala, D. (1999). The influence of sonority on children’s cluster reductions. Journal of Communication Disorders, 32, 397–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, S. (2002). Quantifying the sonority hierarchy. PhD, University of Massachusetts.

  • Parker, S. (2008). Sound level protrusions as physical correlates of sonority. Journal of Phonetics, 36, 55–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pertz, D. L., & Bever, T. G. (1975). Sensitivity to phonological universals in children and adolescents. Language, 51, 149–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pitt, M. A. (1998). Phonological processes and the perception of phonotactically illegal consonant clusters. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 941–951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (1993/2004). Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

  • R Development Core Team. (2011). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. (Version 2.10.1). Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org

  • Redford, M. A. (2008). Production constraints on learning novel onset phonotactics. Cognition, 107, 785–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Romani, C., & Calabrese, A. (1998). Syllabic constraints in the phonological errors of an aphasic patient. Brain and Language, 64, 83–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh PA: Psychology Software Tools.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selkirk, E. O. (1984). On the major class features and syllable theory. In M. Aronoff & R. T. Oerhle (Eds.), Language sound structure (pp. 107–136). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smolensky, P. (2006). Optimality in Phonology II: Harmonic completeness, local constraint conjunction, and feature domain markedness. In P. Smolensky & G. Legendre (Eds.), The harmonic mind: From neural computation to Optimality-theoretic grammar (Linguistic and philosophical implications) (Vol. 2, pp. 27–160). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenneken, P., Bastiaanse, R., Huber, W., & Jacobs, A. M. (2005). Syllable structure and sonority in language inventory and aphasic neologisms. Brain and Language, 95, 280–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steriade, D. (1982). Greek prosodies and the nature of syllabification. PhD, MIT, Cambridge, MA .

  • Stevens, K. N. (1989). On the quantal nature of speech. Journal of Phonetics, 17, 3–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Orden, G. C., Pennington, B. F., & Stone, G. O. (1990). Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics. Psychological Review, 97, 488–522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vitevich, M., & Luce, P. (2005). Increases in phonotactic probability facilitate spoken nonword repetition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 374–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (2004). A web-based interface to calculate phonotactic probability for words and nonwords in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 481–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. (2004). A review of perceptual cues and cue robustness. In D. Steriade, R. M. Kirchner, & B. Hayes (Eds.), Phonetically based phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yavas, M., & Gogate, L. (1999). Phoneme awareness in children: A function of sonority. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 245–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, X., & Berent, I. (2013). Are markedness constraints universal? Evidence from Mandarin Chinese speakers. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Iris Berent.

Appendices

Appendix 1

See Table 11.

Table 11 Monosyllabic items used in the auditory experiments (Experiments 1 and 2)

Appendix 2

See Table 12.

Table 12 Monosyllabic items used in the printed experiment (Experiment 3)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tamási, K., Berent, I. Sensitivity to Phonological Universals: The Case of Stops and Fricatives. J Psycholinguist Res 44, 359–381 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-014-9289-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-014-9289-3

Keywords

Navigation