Skip to main content
Log in

Changing how I feel about the food: experimentally manipulated affective associations with fruits change fruit choice behaviors

  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Fewer than half of Americans meet current recommendations for fruit and vegetable intake. The behavioral affective associations model posits that feelings and emotions associated with a behavior are a proximal influence on decision making. Cross-sectional evidence supports the model and suggests that affective associations predict fruit and vegetable consumption. The purpose of this study was to test whether a causal relation exists between affective associations about fruits and future fruit consumption behavior, as measured by a snack selection task. Following a baseline assessment of cognitive and affective variables, participants’ (N = 161) affective associations about fruits were experimentally manipulated with an implicit priming paradigm. Images of fruits were repeatedly paired with positive, negative, or neutral affective stimuli. The key outcome measure was a behavioral choice task in which participants chose between fruit and a granola bar. Participants in the positive prime condition were three times more likely than those in the negative condition to select a piece of fruit over the granola bar alternative in the snack selection task. They were also twice as likely as those in the neutral condition to select fruit. There were no changes in self-reported affective associations or cognitive beliefs. These findings provide further evidence of the implicit and direct influence of affective associations on behavior, suggesting the need to both incorporate the role of affect in health decision making models, as well as the potential utility of intervention strategies targeting affective associations with health-related behaviors.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Due to small cell sizes for African American, Latin American, and Native American participants, a 3-category race variable was computed and used for hypothesis testing. Categories were: Asian/Pacific Islander (n = 68), white (n = 67), and Other (n = 26).

  2. Results from multinomial logistic regression models that included participants who did not select a snack produced equivalent results, and there were no differences by condition in likelihood of refusing a snack.

  3. Because participants’ accuracy of spacebar responses predicted snack selection, the model was also run with accuracy included as a covariate. A significant condition effect remained, with the positive condition having significantly greater odds of selecting a fruit than the negative condition (OR = 2.70, p < 0.05).

References

  • Ajzen, I., & Driver, B. L. (1991). Prediction of leisure participation from behavioral, normative, and control beliefs: An application of the theory of planned behavior. Leisure Sciences, 13, 185–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I., & Driver, B. L. (1992). Application of the theory of planned behavior to leisure choice. Journal of Leisure Research, 24, 207–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H. T. D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293–1295.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Birch, L. L., & Marlin, D. W. (1982). I don’t like it; I never tried it: Effects of exposure on 2-year-old children’s food preferences. Appetite, 3, 353–360.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Breckler, S. J. (1984). Empirical validation of affect, behavior, and cognition as distinct components of attitude. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1191–1205.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conner, M. T., Perugini, M., O’Gorman, R., Ayres, K., & Prestwich, A. (2007). Relations between implicit and explicit measures of attitudes and measures of behavior: Evidence of moderation by individual difference variables. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1727.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conner, M. T., Rhodes, R. E., Morris, B., McEachan, R., & Lawton, R. (2011). Changing exercise through targeting affective or cognitive attitudes. Psychology and Health, 26, 133–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crites, S. L., Fabrigar, L. R., & Petty, R. E. (1994). Measuring the affective and cognitive properties of attitudes: Conceptual and methodological issues. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 619–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dean, M., Raats, M. M., & Shepherd, R. (2008). Moral concerns and consumer choice of fresh and processed organic foods. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38, 2088–2107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunton, G. F., & Vaughan, E. (2008). Anticipated affective consequences of physical activity adoption and maintenance. Health Psychology, 27, 703–710.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, M. A., & Ainsworth, K. (2012). Predicting university undergraduates’ binge-drinking behavior: A comparative test of the one- and two-component theories of planned behavior. Addictive Behaviors, 37, 92–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer, R. A., Fisher, J. D., Buck, R., & Amico, K. R. (2011). Pilot test of an emotional education intervention component for sexual risk reduction. Health Psychology, 30, 656–660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flay, B. R. (1986). Efficacy and effectiveness trials (and other phases of research) in the development of health promotion programs. Preventive Medicine, 15, 451–474.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friese, M., Hofmann, W., & Wänke, M. (2008). When impulses take over: Moderated predictive validity of explicit and implicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and consumption behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 397–419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, B. (2008). Can evaluative conditioning change attitudes toward mature brands? New evidence from the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 178–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glanz, K., Rimer, B. K., & Lewis, F. M. (2008). Health behavior and health education: Theory, research and practice (4th ed.). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, P., & Cullen, J. W. (1985). The new emphasis in cancer control. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 74, 543–551.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, D., Baeyens, F., Lamote, S., Spruyt, A., & Eelen, P. (2005). Affective priming as an indirect measure of food preferences acquired through odor conditioning. Experimental Psychology, 52, 180–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: Toward an individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 962.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollands, G. J., Prestwich, A., & Marteau, T. M. (2011). Using aversive images to enhance healthy food choices and implicit attitudes: An experimental test of evaluative conditioning. Health Psychology, 30, 195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T. A., Cacioppo, J. T., & Lang, P. J. (1998). Eliciting affect using the international affective picture system: Trajectories through evaluative space. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, W. (2006). DirectRT (Version 2002). [Computer software]. New York, NY: Empirisoft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Askew, W. L., Gillespie, A. H., & Rothman, A. J. (2009). Special issue—Decision making in eating behavior: Interacting perspectives from the individual, family, and environment. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 38, 1–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, C. R., Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2009). Implicit misattribution as a mechanism underlying evaluative conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 933.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keer, M., van den Putte, B., & Neijens, P. (2010). The role of affect and cognition in health decision making. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 49, 143–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kiviniemi, M. T., & Bevins, R. (2007). Affect-behavior associations in motivated behavioral choice: Potential transdisciplinary links. In P. R. Zelick (Ed.), Issues in the psychology of motivation (pp. 65–80). Hauppage, NY: Nova.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiviniemi, M. T., & Bevins, R. (2008). Role of affective associations in the planning and habit systems of decision making related to addiction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 450–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiviniemi, M. T., & Duangdao, K. M. (2009). Affective associations mediate the influence of cost-benefit beliefs on fruit and vegetable consumption. Appetite, 52, 771–775.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kiviniemi, M. T., Voss-Humke, A. M., & Seifert, A. L. (2007). How do I feel about the behavior? The interplay of affective associations with behaviors and cognitive beliefs as influences on physical activity behavior. Health Psychology, 26, 152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W., & Hommer, D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International affective picture system (IAPS): Digitized photographs, instruction manual, and affective ratings (Technical Report A-6). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawton, R., Conner, M., & McEachan, R. (2009). Desire or reason: Predicting health behaviors from affective and cognitive attitudes. Health Psychology, 28, 56–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lawton, R., Conner, M., & Parker, D. (2007). Beyond cognition: Predicting health risk behaviors from instrumental and affective beliefs. Health Psychology, 26, 259–267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ling, A. M., & Horwath, C. (2001). Perceived benefits and barriers of increased fruit and vegetable consumption: Validation of a decisional balance scale. Journal of Nutrition Education, 33, 257–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manstead, A. S. R., & Parker, D. (1995). Evaluating and extending the theory of planned behaviour. European Review of Social Psychology, 6, 69–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Cancer Institute. (2001). Making health communication programs work. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Cancer Institute. (2007). Fruit and vegetable screeners: Scoring the all-day screener. Retrieved Aug 3, 2007, from http://riskfactor.cancer.gov/diet/screeners/fruitveg/scoring/allday.html.

  • O’Carroll, R. E., Foster, C., McGeechan, G., Sandford, K., & Ferguson, E. (2011). The “ick” factor, anticipated regret, and willingness to become an organ donor. Health Psychology, 30, 236–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2001). Implicit attitude formation through classical conditioning. Psychological Science, 12, 413–417.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2002). Implicit acquisition and manifestation of classically conditioned attitudes. Social Cognition, 20, 89–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, M. P., Rogalsky, C., Simmons, A., Feinstein, J. S., & Stein, M. B. (2003). Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism. NeuroImage, 19, 1439–1448.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, B. K., McClernon, F. J., & Dobbins, I. G. (2007). Automatic affective responses to smoking cues. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 15, 400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E., Lipkus, I., & Diefenbach, M. A. (2006). The functions of affect in health communications and in the construction of health preferences. Journal of Communication, 56, S140–S162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pleyers, G., Corneille, O., Luminet, O., & Yzerbyt, V. (2007). Aware and (dis)liking: Item-based analyses reveal that valence acquisition via evaluative conditioning emerges only when there is contingency awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 33, 130–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prestwich, A., Hurling, R., & Baker, S. (2011). Implicit shopping: Attitudinal determinants of the purchasing of healthy and unhealthy foods. Psychology and Health, 26, 875–885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, R. E., & Conner, M. (2010). Comparison of behavioral belief structures in the physical activity domain. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, 2105–2120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, R. E., Fiala, B., & Conner, M. (2009). A review and meta-analysis of affective judgments and physical activity in adult populations. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 38, 180–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richetin, J., Perugini, M., Prestwich, A., & O’Gorman, R. (2007). The IAT as a predictor of food choice: The case of fruits versus snacks. International Journal of Psychology, 42, 166–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P. (2006). The integration of biological, social, cultural, and psychological influences on food choice. In R. Shepherd & M. Raats (Eds.), The psychology of food choice (pp. 19–39). Oxfordshire: CABI.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schulz, K. F., Chalmers, I., Hayes, R. J., & Altman, D. G. (1995). Empirical evidence of bias: Dimensions of methodological quality associated with estimates of treatment effects in controlled trials. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association, 273, 408–412.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simons, J. S., Gaher, R. M., Correia, C. J., Hansen, C. L., & Christopher, M. S. (2005). An affective-motivational model of marijuana and alcohol problems among college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 19, 326–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2007). The affect heuristic. European Journal of Operational Research, 177, 1333–1352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, J., Aronson, E., Crain, A. L., Winslow, M. P., & Fried, C. B. (1994). Inducing hypocrisy as a means of encouraging young adults to use condoms. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 116–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Pligt, J., Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W. W., de Vries, N. K., & Richard, R. (1997). Affect, attitudes and decisions: Let’s be more specific. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 33–66). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhulst, F., Hermans, D., Baeyens, F., Spruyt, A., & Eelen, P. (2006). Determinants and predictive validity of direct and indirect measures of recently acquired food attitudes. Appetite, 46, 137–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wansink, B., Cheney, M. M., & Chan, N. (2003). Exploring comfort food preferences across age and gender. Physiology and Behavior, 79, 739–747.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erin M. Walsh.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Walsh, E.M., Kiviniemi, M.T. Changing how I feel about the food: experimentally manipulated affective associations with fruits change fruit choice behaviors. J Behav Med 37, 322–331 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-012-9490-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-012-9490-5

Keywords

Navigation