Skip to main content
Log in

Hierarchical Processing in ASD is Driven by Exaggerated Salience Effects, not Local Bias

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The role of relative salience in processing of hierarchical stimuli in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) was examined in this study. Participants with ASD and typically developing controls performed a Navon letters task under conditions of global salience, local salience or equal salience of both levels. Results revealed no group differences in level of processing (global or local) and no local bias for ASD. Rather, both groups showed better performance when targets were more salient compared to when distractors were more salient. Importantly, participants with ASD exhibited increased sensitivity to salience at the distractor level. We conclude that inconsistent findings in the context of global/local processing in ASD may stem from such exaggerated salience effects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3 
Fig. 4
Fig. 5 

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. All of the participants were born before 2013 and were diagnosed according to the existing DSM version at the time.

  2. Mostly, only 1–2 trials were excluded (on rare occasions there were three excluded trials) in a block of 48 trials, but never more than that). This was the case for all of the participants, in all conditions, in both groups.

  3. An additional statistical analysis with 'Raven's Progressive Matrices' scores as a co-variate (ANCOVA) replicated the significant level, size and group interaction on IES that were obtained in the original analysis.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington DC: Autor.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington DC: Autor.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington DC: Autor.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baisa, A., Mevorach, C., & Shalev, L. (2019). Can performance in Navon letters among people with autism be affected by saliency? Reexamination of the literature. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 6, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Hoekstra, R. A., Knickmeyer, R., & Wheelwright, S. (2006). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ)-adolescent version. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 343–350.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychologica Belgica, 51, 5–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann, M., Avidan, G., Leonard, G. L., Kimchi, R., Luna, B., Humphreys, K., et al. (2006). Configural processing in autism and its relationship to face processing. Neuropsychologia, 44, 110–129.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Malden: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Granholm, E., Cadenhead, K., Shafer, K. M., & Filoteo, J. V. (2002). Lateralized perceptual organization deficits on the global- local task in schizotypal personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 42–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Granholm, E., Perry, W., Filoteo, J. V., & Braff, D. (1999). Hemispheric and attentional contributions to perceptual organization deficits on the global local task in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 13, 271–281.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guy, J., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Bertone, A. (2019). A developmental perspective of global and local visual perception in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49, 2706–2720.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism and Asperger syndrome faster than normal on the embedded figures test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 527–534.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davis (Eds.), Varieties of attention. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katagiri, M., Kassi, T., Kamio, Y., & Murohashi, H. (2013). Individuals with Asperger's disorder exhibit difficulty in switching attention from a local level to a global level. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 395–403.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimchi, R. (1992). Primacy of wholistic processing and global/local paradigm: A critical review. Psychological Buiietin, 112, 24–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurita, H., Koyama, T., & Osada, H. (2005). Autism-spectrum quotient-Japanese version and its short forms for screening normally intelligent persons with pervasive developmental disorders. Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 59, 490–496.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinchla, R. A., & Wolfe, J. M. (1979). The order of visual processing: "Top-down", "bottom up", or "middle-out". Perception and Psychophysics, 25, 225–231.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., Weigelt, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2013). Global/local processing in autism: Not a disability, but a disinclination. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 2329–2340.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M. R., & Robertson, L. C. (1990). The effect of visual angle on global and local reaction times depends on a set of visual angle presented. Perception and Psychophysics, 47, 489–496.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M. R., & Robertson, L. C. (1989). Do response time advantage and interference reflect the order of processing of global and local level information? Perception and Psychophysics, 46, 254–258.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M. R., Robertson, L. C., & Knight, R. T. (1990). Component mechanisms underlying the processing of hierarchically organized patterns: Inferences from patients with unilateral cortical lesions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 471–483.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, M. K., Crewther, S. G., Blume-Tari, A., Guminsky, M., Perdikeas, N., Roebuck, G., et al. (2002). Temporal processing of global and local information varies with global precedence. Clinical & Experimental Ophthalmology, 30, 221–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leader, G., Loughnane, A., McMoreland, C., & Reed, P. (2009). The effect of stimulus salience on over-selectivity. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 330–338.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCounteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., Dilavore, P. D., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule- generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mevorach, C., Humphreys, G. W., & Shalev, L. (2006). Opposite biases in salience-based selection for the left and right posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 740–742.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mevorach, C., Humphreys, G. W., & Shalev, L. (2009a). Reflexive and preparatory selection and suppression of salient information in the right and left posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1204–1214.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mevorach, C., Shalev, L., Allen, H. A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2009b). The left intraparietal sulcus modulates the selection of low salient stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 303–315.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Belleville, S., & Enns, J. T. (2003). Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high functioning autism: Evidence from multiple paradigms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 904–913.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Stauder, J. E. A., & Robaey, P. (1999). Perceptual processing among high functioning persons with autism. Journal of child Psychology & Psychiatry, 40, 203–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update the eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 27–43.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muth, A., Hönekopp, J., & Falter, C. M. (2014). Visuo-spatial performance in autism: A meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 3245–3263.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1981). The forest revisited: More on global precedence. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 43, 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., O'Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for conjunctive target in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39, 777–783.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Ress, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 733–742.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1979). Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. London: H. K. Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2001). A deficit in shifting attention present in high functioning autism but not Asperger's disorder. Autism, 5, 67–80.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., & Tidswell, T. (1991). The 'windows task' as a measure of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic subjects. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 331–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherf, K. S., Luna, B., Kimchi, R., Minshew, N., & Behrmann, M. (2008). Missing the big picture: Impaired development of global shape processing in autism. Autism Research, 1, 114–129.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schopler, E., Reichler, R., & DeVellis, R. (1980). Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood autism rating scale (CARS). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10, 91–103.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24, 613–620.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shalev, L. (2007). Do local bias and local to global interference reflect intact global processing in autism? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 575–577.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spaniol, M. M., Shalev, L., & Mevorach, C. (2018). Reduced distractor interference in neurotypical adults with high expression of autistic traits irrespective of stimulus type. Autism Research, 11, 1345–1355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, J., Milles, C., Wilson, A. D., Plumb, M. S., & Mon-Williams, M. A. (2009). Slow motor responses to visual stimuli of low salience in autism. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41, 419–426.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Hallen, R., Evers, K., Brewaeys, K., Van den Noortgate, W., & Wagemans, J. (2015). Global processing takes time: A meta-analysis on local-global visual processing in ASD. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 549–573.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L., Mottron, L., Peng, D., Bertiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local to global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time and visual angle. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 550–574.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S., Jiang, M., Duchesne, X. M., Laugeson, E. A., Kennedy, D. P., Adolphs, R., et al. (2015). Atypical visual saliency in autism spectrum disorder quantified through model-based eye tracking. Neuron, 88, 604–616.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Woodbury-Smith, M. R., Robinson, J., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). Screening adults for asperger syndrome using the AQ: A preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 331–335.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was funded by Israel Science Foundation (ISF) Grant No. 1595/11.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

This study was prepared from doctoral dissertation of AB who conceived of the study, designed collected and analyzed the data, and drafted the manuscript; CM participated in the interpretation and conceptualization of the data; LS conceived of the study, and participated in its design, interpretation and conceptualization of the data and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ayelet Baisa.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

All authors declares that he/she has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures with human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Tel Aviv university ethics committee and by the Ministry of Education’s Chief Scientist and with the 1964 Heisinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (XLS 62 kb)

Supplementary file2 (DOCX 23 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Baisa, A., Mevorach, C. & Shalev, L. Hierarchical Processing in ASD is Driven by Exaggerated Salience Effects, not Local Bias. J Autism Dev Disord 51, 666–676 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04578-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04578-1

Keywords

Navigation