Skip to main content
Log in

Differences in Neural Correlates of Speech Perception in 3 Month Olds at High and Low Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this study, we investigated neural precursors of language acquisition as potential endophenotypes of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in 3-month-old infants at high and low familial ASD risk. Infants were imaged using functional near-infrared spectroscopy while they listened to auditory stimuli containing syllable repetitions; their neural responses were analyzed over left and right temporal regions. While female low risk infants showed initial neural activation that decreased over exposure to repetition-based stimuli, potentially indicating a habituation response to repetition in speech, female high risk infants showed no changes in neural activity over exposure. This finding may indicate a potential neural endophenotype of language development or ASD specific to females at risk for the disorder.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The association between sex and ASD differs with cognitive ability. In children who are cognitively high functioning, the sex ratio may be more than 5.5:1 (M:F), while in children with intellectual disability (ID), the ASD sex ratio is closer to 2:1 (Newschaffer et al. 2007).

  2. Hair, particularly that which is highly pigmented, interferes with NIRS measurements by absorbing near-infrared (NIR) light from source optodes before it penetrates the skull and arrives at the brain tissue. NIRS headgear is thus designed to keep hair out of the path of NIR light as much as possible. Many adaptations for reducing interference by hair may cause discomfort however, making them inappropriate for studies on infant populations.

  3. These regions of interest (ROIs) cover roughly identical topographies as those used by Gervain et al. (2008). We include fewer channels in each ROI average herein, in order to distinguish regional brain activity more clearly than was perhaps possible in Gervain’s study of newborns (who have smaller average head sizes than 3 month olds) and to maintain consistency with other studies conducted in our lab, using the same NIRS headgear (i.e., Keehn et al. 2013; Wagner et al. 2011).

  4. Mauchly’s test of sphericity indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated for the main effect of region, χ2(5) = 27.40, p < 0.001. Therefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.64 for the main effect of region).

  5. Mauchly’s test of sphericity indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated for the main effect of region, χ2(5) = 18.33, p = 0.003. Therefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = 0.705 for the main effect of region).

  6. The exposure effects herein are likely not an artifact of the number of trials that participants heard, as the majority (n = 33; 87%) of participants listened to all 28 trials, 2 participants (5.3%) stopped after 25 trials, 1 (2.6%) stopped after 24 trials, 1 (2.6%) stopped after 23 trials, and 1 (2.6%) stopped after 19 trials. Additionally, the number of trials heard did not differ significantly between risk groups (p = 0.18), genders (p = 0.65), or syllabic sequence type (p = 1.00). Finally, the four-way interaction between exposure, syllabic sequence type, risk group and gender was robust to the inclusion of number of trials heard as a covariate in the mixed-design (2 × 4 × 2 × 2x2) ANOVA: exposure × syllabic sequence type × risk group × gender (F(1,32) = 5.46, p = 0.026, partial η2 = 0.146); all interactions with number of trials (p > 0.05).

  7. At the request of a reviewer, we conducted a 2 (syllabic sequence type) × 4 (region) × 2 (risk group) × 2 (exposure) mixed-design ANOVA, to examine exposure results collapsing across gender. This analysis revealed that the main effect of region was significant when gender was not included as a between-subjects factor (F(2.09, 75.15, corrected using Greenhouse–Geisser estimate of sphericity (ε= 0.696)) = 3.45, p = 0.046, partial η2 = 0.081). Additionally, elimination of HRA females from the sample extinguished the main effect of region over exposure (F(3,24) = 0.591, p = 0.627).

  8. Preliminary analyses of this nature—based on a subset of the present sample—suggest that the magnitude of the fNIRS neural response to the last four ABB stimuli predicts MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory-III (Fenson et al. 2007) gesture production (p < 0.001, R2 = 0.7005) and sentence complexity (p < 0.001, R2 = 0.4999), and Mullen Scales of Early Learning (Mullen 1995) expressive language scores (p < 0.001, R2 = 0.5052) for both LRC and HRA subjects at 18 months, across gender.

  9. It is also possible that the gender effects found herein are confounding results that are instead attributable to participants’ eventual ASD outcomes (since more males than females will go on to develop ASD). Unfortunately, we did not have the statistical power to test this hypothesis directly, as only 19 of the 38 infants (11 LRC, 8 HRA) analyzed herein returned for clinical evaluation and diagnosis confirmation (or lack thereof) at 36 months of age.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baio, J., 010 Principal, et al. (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years-autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Surveillance Summaries (Washington, DC: 2002), 63(2), 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, D. J., Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (2002). Alternative approaches to analyzing individual differences in the rate of early vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics, doi:10.1017/S0142716402003016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boddaert, N., Belin, P., Chabane, N., Poline, J.-B., Barthélémy, C., Mouren-Simeoni, M.-C., … Zilbovicius, M. (2003). Perception of complex sounds: abnormal pattern of cortical activation in autism. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(11), 2057–2060.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., & Haynes, O. M. (2004). Specific and general language performance across early childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First Language, 24(3), 267–304. doi:10.1177/0142723704045681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosl, W., Tierney, A., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. (2011). EEG complexity as a biomarker for autism spectrum disorder risk. BMC Medicine, 9, 18. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-9-18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2013). Lexicon structure and the disambiguation of novel words: Evidence from bilingual infants. Cognition, 128(3), 407–416. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.05.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Academic Press.

  • De Giacomo, A., & Fombonne, E. (1998). Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 7(3), 131–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Devor, A., Ulbert, I., Dunn, A. K., Narayanan, S. N., Jones, S. R., Andermann, M. L., … Dale, A. M. (2005). Coupling of the cortical hemodynamic response to cortical and thalamic neuronal activity. In Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(10), 3822–3827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elsabbagh, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2010). Getting answers from babies about autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(2), 81–87. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsabbagh, M., Volein, A., Csibra, G., Holmboe, K., Garwood, H., Tucker, L., … Johnson, M. H. (2009). Neural correlates of eye gaze processing in the infant broader autism phenotype. Biological Psychiatry, 65(1), 31–38. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.034.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emberson, L. L., Cannon, G., Palmeri, H., Richards, J. E., & Aslin, R. N. (2017). Using fNIRS to examine occipital and temporal responses to stimulus repetition in young infants: Evidence of selective frontal cortex involvement. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 26–38. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2016.11.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pérez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., … Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(Pt 2), 326–343. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eyler, L. T., Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2012). A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 135(3), 949–960. doi:10.1093/brain/awr364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: Decreased attention to familiar patterns relative to novel ones. Science, 146(3644), 668–670.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flagg, E. J., Cardy, J. E. O., Roberts, W., & Roberts, T. P. L. (2005). Language lateralization development in children with autism: Insights from the late field magnetoencephalogram. Neuroscience Letters, 386(2), 82–87. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.05.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, S. E., Wagner, J. B., Shrock, C. L., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2013). Neural processing of facial identity and emotion in infants at high-risk for autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 89. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00089.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gabard-Durnam, L., Tierney, A. L., Vogel-Farley, V., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2015). Alpha asymmetry in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(2), 473–480. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1926-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gervain, J., Macagno, F., Cogoi, S., Peña, M., & Mehler, J. (2008). The neonate brain detects speech structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(37), 14222–14227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gleason, J. B., & Ely, R. (2002). Gender differences in language development. Biology, Society, and Behavior: The Development of Sex Differences in Cognition, 21, 127–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottesman, I. I., & Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(4), 636–645.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grill-Spector, K., Henson, R., & Martin, A. (2006). Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(1), 14–23. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guiraud, J. A., Kushnerenko, E., Tomalski, P., Davies, K., Ribeiro, H., Johnson, M. H., & BASIS Team. (2011). Differential habituation to repeated sounds in infants at high risk for autism. Neuroreport, 22(16), 845–849. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834c0bec.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halladay, A. K., Bishop, S., Constantino, J. N., Daniels, A. M., Koenig, K., Palmer, K., … Szatmari, P. (2015). Sex and gender differences in autism spectrum disorder: summarizing evidence gaps and identifying emerging areas of priority. Molecular Autism. doi:10.1186/s13229-015-0019-y.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Huppert, T. J., Diamond, S. G., Franceschini, M. A., & Boas, D. A. (2009). HomER: A review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Applied Optics, 48(10), D280–D298.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37(2), 229–261. doi:10.1017/S0305000909990432.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kameyama, M., Fukuda, M., Uehara, T., & Mikuni, M. (2004). Sex and age dependencies of cerebral blood volume changes during cognitive activation: a multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy study. NeuroImage, 22(4), 1715–1721.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keehn, B., Vogel-Farley, V., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2015). Atypical hemispheric specialization for faces in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 8(2), 187–198.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Keehn, B., Wagner, J. B., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2013). Functional connectivity in the first year of life in infants at-risk for autism: a preliminary near-infrared spectroscopy study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 444. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00444.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Key, A. P. F., & Stone, W. L. (2012). Same but different: 9-month-old infants at average and high risk for autism look at the same facial features but process them using different brain mechanisms. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 5(4), 253–266. doi:10.1002/aur.1231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinhans, N. M., Müller, R.-A., Cohen, D. N., & Courchesne, E. (2008). Atypical functional lateralization of language in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research, 1221, 115–125. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.04.080.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Knaus, T. A., Silver, A. M., Kennedy, M., Lindgren, K. A., Dominick, K. C., Siegel, J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2010). Language laterality in autism spectrum disorder and typical controls: A functional, volumetric, and diffusion tensor MRI study. Brain and Language, 112(2), 113–120. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(11), 831–843. doi:10.1038/nrn1533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, M.-C., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. N. V., Wheelwright, S. J., Sadek, S. A., … Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020835.

    Google Scholar 

  • Little, V. M., Thomas, D. G., & Letterman, M. R. (1999). Single-trial analyses of developmental trends in infant auditory event-related potentials. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16(3), 455–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd-Fox, S., Blasi, A., Elwell, C. E., Charman, T., Murphy, D., & Johnson, M. H. (2013). Reduced neural sensitivity to social stimuli in infants at risk for autism. Proceedings of Royal Society B, 280(1758), 20123026. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.3026.

  • Lombardo, M. V., Pierce, K., Eyler, L. T., Carter Barnes, C., Ahrens-Barbeau, C., Solso, S., … Courchesne, E. (2015). Different functional neural substrates for good and poor language outcome in autism. Neuron, 86(2), 567–577. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.023.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Luyster, R. J., Powell, C., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2014). Neural measures of social attention across the first years of life: characterizing typical development and markers of autism risk. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 131–143. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2013.09.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Rao, S. B., & Vishton, P. M. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science, 283(5398), 77–80. doi:10.1126/science.283.5398.77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCleery, J. P., Akshoomoff, N., Dobkins, K. R., & Carver, L. J. (2009). Atypical face versus object processing and hemispheric asymmetries in 10-month-old infants at risk for autism. Biological Psychiatry, 66(10), 950–957. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.031.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Messinger, D., Young, G. S., Ozonoff, S., Dobkins, K., Carter, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., … Sigman, M. (2013). Beyond autism: A baby siblings research consortium study of high-risk children at three years of age. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(3), 300–308.e1. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.12.011.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning (AGS ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newschaffer, C. J., Croen, L. A., Daniels, J., Giarelli, E., Grether, J. K., Levy, S. E., et al. (2007). The epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders*. Annual Review of Public Health, 28, 235–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Okamoto, M., Dan, H., Sakamoto, K., Takeo, K., Shimizu, K., Kohno, S., … Dan, I. (2004). Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping. NeuroImage, 21(1), 99–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D., Yirmiya, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., … Stone, W. L. (2011). Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics, 128(3), e488–e495. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-2825.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Parke, R., & Gauvain, M. (2008). Child psychology: a contemporary view point (7 ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peña, M., Maki, A., Kovacić, D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Koizumi, H., Bouquet, F., & Mehler, J. (2003). Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(20), 11702–11705. doi:10.1073/pnas.1934290100.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Perani, D., Saccuman, M. C., Scifo, P., Anwander, A., Spada, D., Baldoli, C., … Friederici, A. D. (2011). Neural language networks at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(38), 16056–16061. doi:10.1073/pnas.1102991108.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Righi, G., Tierney, A. L., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2014). Functional connectivity in the first year of life in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: An EEG study. PLoS ONE, 9(8), e105176. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0105176.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Seery, A., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2014). Event-related potentials to repeated speech in 9-month-old infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 6(1), 43. doi:10.1186/1866-1955-6-43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Seery, A. M., Vogel-Farley, V., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2013). Atypical lateralization of ERP response to native and non-native speech in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 10–24. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, P., Kjelgaard, M. M., Gandhi, T. K., Tsourides, K., Cardinaux, A. L., Pantazis, D., … Held, R. M. (2014). Autism as a disorder of prediction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 111(42), 15220–15225. doi:10.1073/pnas.1416797111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96, 1. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (2010). The origins of social impairments in autism spectrum disorder: Studies of infants at risk. Neural Networks, 23(8–9), 1072–1076. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2010.07.008.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. G., & Lykins, M. S. (1995). Event-related potential measures of 24-hour retention in 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 946.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tierney, A. L., Gabard-Durnam, L., Vogel-Farley, V., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2012). Developmental trajectories of resting EEG power: An endophenotype of autism spectrum disorder. PLoS ONE, 7(6), e39127. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039127.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tong, Y., & Frederick, B. D. (2010). Time lag dependent multimodal processing of concurrent fMRI and near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) data suggests a global circulatory origin for low-frequency oscillation signals in human brain. Neuroimage, 53, 553–564. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.049.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Turk-Browne, N. B., Scholl, B. J., & Chun, M. M. (2008). Babies and brains: habituation in infant cognition and functional neuroimaging. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.016.2008.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vander Maelen, A. L., Strauss, M. E., & Starr, R. H. (1975). Influence of obstetric medication on auditory habituation in the newborn. Developmental Psychology, 11(6), 711–714. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.11.6.711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vannasing, P., Florea, O., González-Frankenberger, B., Tremblay, J., Paquette, N., Safi, D., … Gallagher, A. (2016). Distinct hemispheric specializations for native and non-native languages in one-day-old newborns identified by fNIRS. Neuropsychologia, 84, 63–69. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.038.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Viding, E., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2007). Endophenotype approach to developmental psychopathology: Implications for autism research. Behavior Genetics, 37(1), 51–60. doi:10.1007/s10519-006-9105-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, J. B., Fox, S. E., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2011). Neural processing of repetition and non-repetition grammars in 7- and 9-month-old infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 168. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00168.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wan, C. Y., Marchina, S., Norton, A., & Schlaug, G. (2012). Atypical hemispheric asymmetry in the arcuate fasciculus of completely nonverbal children with autism. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252(1), 332–337. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06446.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe, A., & Kato, T. (2004). Cerebrovascular response to cognitive tasks in patients with schizophrenia measured by near-infrared spectroscopy. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30(2), 435–444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A., Woods, J., Allen, L., Cleary, J., Dickinson, H., & Lord, C. (2004). Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 473–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, J. J., Gerig, G., Lewis, J. D., Soda, T., Styner, M. A., Vachet, C., … IBIS Network. (2015). Altered corpus callosum morphology associated with autism over the first 2 years of life. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 138(Pt 7), 2046–2058. doi:10.1093/brain/awv118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 143–152. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to the families for their invaluable contribution to the Infant Sibling Project. We would also like to acknowledge the Infant Sibling Project staff—Tara Augenstein, Lauren Baczewski, Leah Casner, Kristin Concannon, Frances Cooley, Morgan Crossman, Kerri Downing, Mary Kate Driscoll, Sharon Fox, Brandon Keehn, Jack Keller, Nina Leezenbaum, Vanessa Loukas, Rhiannon Luyster, Stephanie Marshall, Sarah Mumanachit, Anne Seery, Meagan Thompson, Vanessa Vogel-Farley, and Anne-Marie Zuluaga—for their assistance in data acquisition.

Funding

This research was supported by NIH R01-DC010290 to Helen Tager-Flusberg and Charles A. Nelson and The Simons Foundation (137186) to Charles A. Nelson.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LAE, JBW, HTF, and CAN conceived of the study and its design. LAE processed, analyzed and interpreted the data, and drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed intellectual content and critical revisions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Charles A. Nelson.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interests.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. All study procedures were monitored and approved by the Boston Children’s Hospital Institutional Review Board under IRB protocol X10-02-0083.

Additional information

This paper was prepared from the doctoral dissertation of Laura A. Edwards (2015). It was presented as a poster at the 2016 International Meeting for Autism Research in Baltimore, MD, USA, on May 14, 2016. It is part of a larger longitudinal investigation (“The Infant Sibling Project”) conducted at Boston Children’s Hospital and Boston University.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 228 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Edwards, L.A., Wagner, J.B., Tager-Flusberg, H. et al. Differences in Neural Correlates of Speech Perception in 3 Month Olds at High and Low Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 47, 3125–3138 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3222-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3222-1

Keywords

Navigation