Skip to main content
Log in

How Effective is Example Generation for Learning Declarative Concepts?

  • Replication
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Declarative concepts (i.e., key terms and corresponding definitions for abstract concepts) represent foundational knowledge that students learn in many content domains. Thus, investigating techniques to enhance concept learning is of critical importance. Various theoretical accounts support the expectation that example generation will serve this purpose, but few studies have examined the efficacy of this technique. We conducted three experiments involving 487 undergraduates to investigate the effects of example generation on concept learning and examined factors that may moderate its effectiveness. Students read a short text that introduced eight concepts. Some students were then prompted to generate concrete examples of each concept followed by definition restudy, whereas others only restudied definitions for the same amount of time. Two days later, students completed final tests involving example generation and definition cued recall. Meta-analytic outcomes indicated that example generation yields moderate improvements in learning of declarative concepts, relative to restudy only. Each experiment also included additional groups to investigate potential moderators. Example generation tended to be more effective with spaced versus massed restudy. Despite strong correlations between the quality of examples generated during practice and final test performance, experimental manipulations that improved example quality did not improve learning. In sum, the current work establishes that example generation enhances concept learning and provides an important foundation for further investigating factors that moderate its benefits to learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We selected definition restudy as the activity for the comparison group (as opposed to no practice or some other encoding strategy) because it arguably provides the most appropriate business-as-usual comparison, given that restudy is the technique that students often report using most frequently during self-regulated learning (e.g., Hartwig and Dunlosky 2012; Karpicke et al. 2009; Susser and McCabe 2013). To foreshadow, experiments 2 and 3 also afforded comparison of example generation to another learning technique (retrieval practice).

  2. On average, participants spent 2.7 min (SE = 0.1) studying the text and 13.3 s (SE = 0.7) per concept definition; similar outcomes were observed in experiments 2 and 3 [text M = 2.8 min (SE = 0.1) for the text and M = 12.7 s (SE = 0.5) per concept definition in experiment 2; M = 2.7 min (SE = 0.1) for the text and M = 11.6 s (SE = 0.5) per concept definition in experiment 3]. Neither text study time nor concept study time differed significantly as a function of group in any experiment, and including these variables as covariates in analyses of final test performance did not qualitatively change any statistical conclusions.

  3. Given that timing of restudy did not significantly moderate the benefit of example generation over restudy-only in experiment 1, we dropped the timing manipulation from experiment 2 to keep the design from becoming too unwieldy with the addition of other extension groups. Given the advantage of example generation involving spaced versus massed restudy, we used spaced restudy for all groups in experiments 2 and 3.

  4. Cognitive overload from completing both tasks was unlikely, given that learners were prompted to complete the two tasks sequentially, spent a similar amount of time for each component task as learners in the single-task groups (example generation only or recall only; see Table 5), and produced responses of similar quality as learners in the single-task groups (see Tables 3 and 4).

  5. Simmons et al. (2011) particularly recommend that the field “should be more tolerant of imperfections in results … Underpowered studies with perfect results are the ones that should invite extra scrutiny.”

References

  • Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 612–637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaxton, T. A. (1989). Investigating dissociations among memory measures: support for a transfer-appropriate processing framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 657–668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blunt, J. R., & Karpicke, J. D. (2014). Learning with retrieval-based concept mapping. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 849–858.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braver, S. L., Thoemmes, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (2014). Continuously cumulating meta-analysis and replicability. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 333–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2008). Feedback enhances the positive effects and reduces the negative effects multiple-choice testing. Memory & Cognition, 36, 604–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, A. C., Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2007). The effect of type and timing of feedback on learning from multiple-choice tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 273–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chan, J. C. K. (2009). When does retrieval induce forgetting and when does it induce facilitation? Implications for retrieval inhibition, testing effect, and text processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 153–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Roscoe, R. D., Slotta, J. D., Roy, M., & Chase, C. C. (2012). Misconceived causal explanations for emergent processes. Cognitive Science, 36, 1–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coane, J. H. (2013). Retrieval practice and elaborative encoding benefit memory in younger and older adults. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 2, 95–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cortina, J. M., & Nouri, H. (2000). Effect size for ANOVA designs. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • de Bruin, A. B. H., Thiede, K. W., Camp, G., & Redford, J. (2011). Generating keywords improves metacomprehension and self-regulation in elementary and middle school children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 294–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dornisch, M., Sperling, R. A., & Zeruth, J. A. (2011). The effects of levels of elaboration on learners’ strategic processing of text. Instructional Science, 39, 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J & Ariel, R. (2011). Self-regulated learning and the allocation of study time. In B. Ross (Ed), Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 54, 103–140.

  • Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., & Middleton, E. L. (2005). What constrains the accuracy of metacomprehension judgments? Testing the transfer-appropriate-monitoring and accessibility hypotheses. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 551–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., Hartwig, M. K., Rawson, K. A., & Lipko, A. R. (2011). Improving college students’ evaluation of text learning using idea-unit standards. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 467–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14, 4–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Owen, P. D., & Cote, N. C. (1990). Encoding and recall of texts: the importance of material appropriate processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 5, 566–581.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goossens, N. A. M. C., Camp, G., Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L., Tabbers, H. K., & Zwaan, R. A. (2014). The benefit of retrieval practice over elaborative restudy in primary school vocabulary learning. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3, 177–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gorrell, J., Tricou, C., & Graham, A. (1991). Children’s short- and long-term retention of science concepts via self-generated examples. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 5, 100–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurung, R. A. R. (2005). How do students really study (and does it matter)? Teaching of Psychology, 32, 239–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurung, R. A. R., Weidert, J., & Jeske, A. (2010). Focusing on how students study. Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 10, 28–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, R. J. (1989). The effects of learner-generated elaborations on concept learning from prose. The Journal of Experimental Education, 57, 205–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, R. J. (1990). The effect of elaboration on the acquisition of conceptual problem-solving skills from prose. The Journal of Experimental Education, 59, 5–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, R. J. (1997). Effects of three types of elaboration on learning concepts from text. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 22, 299–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, R. J. (1999). The role of elaboration within a text processing and text adjunct context. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 363–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, R. J. (2004). Material appropriate processing and elaboration: the impact of balanced and complementary types of processing on learning concepts from text. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 221–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartwig, M. K., & Dunlosky, J. (2012). Study strategies of college students: are self-testing and scheduling related to achievement? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 126–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., & McClelland, G. H. (1989). Data analysis: a model-comparison approach. San Diego: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Tuovinen, J., & Sweller, J. (2001). When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 579–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Rikers, R., & Paas, F. (2012). Educational implications of expertise reversal effects in learning and performance of complex cognitive and sensorimotor skills. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 313–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpicke, J. D., & Blunt, J. R. (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborate studying with concept mapping. Science, 331, 772–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpicke, J. D., Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2009). Metacognitive strategies in student learning: do students practice retrieval when they study on their own? Memory, 17, 471–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornell, N., Bjork, R. A., & Garcia, M. A. (2011). Why tests appear to prevent forgetting: a distribution-based bifurcation model. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 85–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lishner, D. A. (2015). A concise set of core recommendations to promote the dependability of psychological research. Review of General Psychology, 19, 52–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maner, J. K. (2014). Let’s put our money where our mouth is: if authors are to change their ways, reviewers (and editors) must change with them. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 343–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcafle, J., & Finn, B. (2008). Evidence that judgments of learning are causally related to study choices. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 174–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Kornell, N. (2005). A region of proximal learning model of study time allocation. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 463–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., Dunlosky, J., Graf, A., & Narens, L. (1994). Utilization of metacognitive judgments in the allocation of study during multitrial learning. Psychological Science, 5, 207–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuschatz, J. S., Preston, E. L., Toglia, M. P., & Neuschatz, J. S. (2005). Comparison of the efficacy of two name-learning techniques: expanding rehearsal and name-face imagery. American Journal of Psychology, 118, 79–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F. G. W. C., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: a cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., & Harris, C. R. (2012). Is the replicability crisis overblown? Three arguments examined. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 531–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., Rohrer, D., Cepeda, N. J., & Carpenter, S. K. (2007). Enhancing learning and retarding forgetting: choices and consequences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 187–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A. (2012). Why do rereading lag effects depend on test delay? Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 870–884.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2007). Improving self-evaluation of learning for key concepts in expository texts. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 559–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2011). Optimizing schedules of retrieval practice for durable and efficient learning: how much is enough? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 283–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (2005). Rereading effects depend upon time of test. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 70–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A., Thomas, R. C., & Jacoby, L. L. (2015). The power of examples: illustrative examples enhance conceptual learning of declarative concepts. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 483–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (2014). Toward an instructionally oriented theory of example-based learning. Cognitive Science, 38, 1–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. (1990). Implicit memory: retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45, 1043–1056.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. I. I. I., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning: taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17, 249–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowland, C. A. (2014). The effect of testing versus restudy on retention: a meta-analytic review of the testing effect. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1432–1463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, e22, 1359–1366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J. (2014). The value of direct replication. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 76–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, D. J., & Spence, J. R. (2014). Expectations for replications: are yours realistic? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 305–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Susser, J. A., & McCabe, J. (2013). From the lab to the dorm room: metacognitive awareness and use of spaced study. Instructional Science, 41, 345–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Dunlosky, J., Griffin, T. D., & Wiley, J. (2005). Understanding the delayed keyword effect on metacomprehension accuracy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 1267–1280.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Loon, M. H., de Bruin, A. B. H., van Gog, T., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2013). The effect of delayed-JOLs and sentence generation on children’s monitoring accuracy and regulation of idiom study. Metacognition and Learning, 8, 173–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, Y., Lawrence, J. S., Tran, N., & Frye, A. A. (2013). How and how much do students study? Tracking study habits with the diary method. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Toronto, Canada.

  • Winne, P. H., & Hadwin, A. F. (1998). Studying as self-regulated learning. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 277–304). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woloshyn, V. E., & Stockley, D. B. (1995). Helping students acquire belief-inconsistent and belief-consistent science facts: comparisons between individual and dyad study using elaborative interrogation, self-selected study and repetitious-reading. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 75–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, E., Willoughby, T., Kaspar, V., & Idle, T. (1994). Enhancing adolescents’ recall of factual content: the impact of provided versus self-generated elaborations. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 40, 57–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wooldridge, C. L., Bugg, J. M., McDaniel, M. A., & Liu, Y. (2014). The testing effect with authentic educational materials: a cautionary note. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3, 214–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zamary, A., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2015). How accurately can students evaluate the quality of self-generated examples of declarative concepts? Not well, and feedback does not help. Submitted manuscript.

Download references

Acknowledgments

The research reported here was supported by a James S. McDonnell Foundation 21st Century Science Initiative in Bridging Brain, Mind and Behavior Collaborative Award.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Katherine A. Rawson.

Appendix: Excerpt from Text and Concept Definitions Used in Experiments 1–3

Appendix: Excerpt from Text and Concept Definitions Used in Experiments 1–3

Although attribution often involves the logical kind of reasoning just described, this is not always the case. In fact, it is subject to several kinds of biases. One of the most important is known as the correspondence bias, which is the tendency to attribute other people’s behavior to internal causes to a greater extent than is actually justified while underestimating the effect of the situation. The correspondence bias can lead us to false conclusions about others. Another bias in our attributions concerns our own behavior. The self-serving bias is the tendency to attribute positive outcomes to our own traits or characteristics (internal causes) but negative outcomes to factor beyond our control (external causes). Finally, the just-world hypothesis refers to the strong desire or need people have to believe that the world is an orderly, predictable, and just place, where people get what they deserve. This influences our attributions because when we encounter evidence suggesting that the world is not just, we sometimes persuade ourselves that no injustice has occurred.

Correspondence bias

The tendency to attribute other people’s behavior to internal causes to a greater extent than is actually justified while underestimating the effect of the situation

Self-serving bias

The tendency to attribute positive outcomes to our own traits or characteristics (internal causes) but negative outcomes to factor beyond our control (external causes)

Just-world hypothesis

The strong desire or need people have to believe that the world is an orderly, predictable, and just place, where people get what they deserve

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rawson, K.A., Dunlosky, J. How Effective is Example Generation for Learning Declarative Concepts?. Educ Psychol Rev 28, 649–672 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-016-9377-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-016-9377-z

Keywords

Navigation