Skip to main content
Log in

Anuran predators overcome visual illusion: dazzle coloration does not protect moving prey

  • Short Communication
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Predators everywhere impose strong selection pressures on the morphology and behavior of their prey, but the resulting antipredator adaptations vary greatly among species. Studies of adaptive coloration in prey species have generally focused on cryptic or aposematic prey, with little consideration of color patterns in palatable mobile prey. Complex color patterns have been proposed to decrease the ability of visual predators to capture moving prey (motion dazzle effect). Most support for this hypothesis, however, comes from experiments with human subjects and simulated prey. We tested the motion dazzle effect using, for the first time, natural predators (cane toads, Rhinella marina) and live prey (house crickets, Acheta domesticus) with altered color patterns. We found no support for the motion dazzle effect as striped crickets did not fare better than solid colored ones. Crickets that spent more time moving, however, were more likely to be eaten. Our results suggest that motion specialized visual predators such as toads overcome the motion dazzle effect and impose stronger selection pressure on prey behavior than on coloration. These findings emphasize the importance of sensory specializations of predators in mediating antipredator strategies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Data availability

The datasets generated and analyzed during this study are available from the Dryad Data Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.gq6326t.

References

  • Allen WL, Baddeley R, Scott-Samuel NE, Cuthill IC (2013) The evolution and function of pattern diversity in snakes. Behav Ecol 24:1237–1250

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67:1–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum-Conradi H, Ewert JP (1999) Responses of single neurons in the toad’s caudal ventral striatum to moving visual stimuli and test of their efferent projection by extracellular antidromic stimulation/recording techniques. Brain Behav Evol 54:338–354

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dusenbery DB (1992) Sensory ecology: how animals acquire and respond to information. W.H. Freeman, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Forsman A, Appelqvist S (1998) Visual predators impose correlation selection on prey color pattern and behavior. Behav Ecol 9:409–413

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall JR, Cuthill IC, Baddeley R, Shohet AJ, Scott-Samuel NE (2013) Camouflage, detection and identification of moving targets. Proc R Soc B 280:20130064

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hämäläinen L, Valkonen J, Mappes J, Rojas B (2015) Visual illusions in predator-prey interactions: birds find moving patterned prey harder to catch. Anim Cogn 18:1059–1068

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hogan BG, Cuthill IC, Scott-Samuel NE (2016) Dazzle camouflage, target tracking, and the confusion effect. Behav Ecol 27:1547–1551

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes AE, Troscianko J, Stevens M (2014) Motion dazzle and the effects of target patterning on capture success. BMC Evol Biol 14:201

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes AE, Magor-Elliott RS, Stevens M (2015) The role of stripe orientation in target capture success. Front Zool 12:17

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson JF, Ingram W, Campbell HW (1976) The dorsal pigmentation pattern of snakes as an antipredator strategy: a multivariate approach. Am Nat 110:1029–1053

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meshaka WE, Powell R (2010) Diets of the native southern toad (Anaxyrus terrestris) and the exotic cane toad (Rhinella marina) from a single site in south-central Florida. Fla Sci 73:175–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Pough FH (1976) Multiple cryptic effects of crossbanded and ringed patterns of snakes. Copeia 1976:834–836

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team (2017) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruxton GD, Sherratt TN, Speed MP (2004) Avoiding Attack: the evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals, and mimicry. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Santer RD (2013) Motion dazzle: a locust’s eye view. Biol Lett 9:20130811

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Satou M, Shiraishi A (1991) Local motion processing in the optic tectum of the Japanese toad, Bufo japonicus. J Comp Physiol A 169:569–589

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Scott-Samuel NE, Baddeley R, Palmer CE, Cuthill IC (2011) Dazzle camouflage affects speed perception. PLoS ONE 6:2–7

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens M (2007) Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of protective coloration. Proc R Soc B 274:1457–1464

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens M, Merilaita S (2011) Animal camouflage: mechanisms and function. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens M, Yule DH, Ruxton GD (2008) Dazzle coloration and prey movement. Proc R Soc B 275:2639–2643

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thayer GH (1909) Concealing-coloration in the animal kingdom: an exposition of the laws of disguise through color and pattern: being a summary of Abbott H. Thayer’s discoveries. Macmillan, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Therneau TM (2015) coxme: mixed effects cox models. R package version 2.2-5. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=coxme. Accessed 20 Oct 2017

  • Zug GR, Zug PB (1979) The marine toad, Bufo marinus: a natural history resumé of native populations. Smithson Contrib Zool 284:1–58

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to J. Lam, N. Anderson, and C. Pantoja, for running experimental trials, R. Lim and E. Shank for coding videos, and J. Peniston for designing figures. We also thank H. Legett, B. Leavell, and F. Boyd for collecting the cane toads used in this study, and D. Pita for measuring the reflectance spectra of the crickets. Members of the Bernal lab provided suggestions that helped improve this manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the Department of Biological Sciences at Purdue University. X.E.B. was funded by NSF IOS no. 1433990.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sara Zlotnik.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical approval

All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. Our experiments were approved by the Purdue Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol #1405001073). This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (PDF 219 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zlotnik, S., Darnell, G.M. & Bernal, X.E. Anuran predators overcome visual illusion: dazzle coloration does not protect moving prey. Anim Cogn 21, 729–733 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-018-1199-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-018-1199-6

Keywords

Navigation