Skip to main content
Log in

Temporal error monitoring with directional error magnitude judgements: a robust phenomenon with no effect of being watched

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A key aspect of metacognition is the ability to monitor performance. A recent line of work has shown that error-monitoring ability captures both the magnitude and direction of timing errors, thereby pointing at the metric composition of error monitoring [e.g., Akdoğan and Balcı (J Exp Psychol https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000265, 2017)]. These studies, however, primarily used a composite variable that combined isolated measures of ordinal confidence ratings (as a proxy for error magnitude judgement) and “shorter/longer than the target” judgements. In two experiments we tested temporal error monitoring (TEM) performance with a more direct measure of directional error magnitude rating on a continuum. The second aim of this study is to test if TEM performance is modulated by the feeling of being watched that was previously shown to influence metacognitive-like monitoring processes. We predicted that being watched would improve TEM performance, particularly in participants with high timing precision (a proxy for high task mastery), and disrupt TEM performance in participants with low timing precision (a proxy for low task mastery). In both experiments, we found strong evidence for TEM ability. However, we did not find any reliable effect of the social stimulus on TEM performance. In short, our results demonstrate that metric error monitoring is a robust metacognitive phenomenon, which is not sensitive to social influence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aiello, J. R., & Douthitt, E. A. (2001). Social facilitation from Triplett to electronic performance monitoring. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 5(3), 163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akdoğan, B., & Balcı, F. (2017). Are you early or late?: Temporal error monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(3), 347–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baltazar, M., Hazem, N., Vilarem, E., Beaucousin, V., Picq, J. L., & Conty, L. (2014). Eye contact elicits bodily self-awareness in human adults. Cognition, 133(1), 120–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brocas, I., Carrillo, J. D., & Tarrasó, J. (2018). Self-awareness of biases in time perception. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 148, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Çavdaroğlu, B., Zeki, M., & Balcı, F. (2014). Time-based reward maximization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0461.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chib, V. S., Adachi, R., & O’doherty, J. P. (2018). Neural substrates of social facilitation effects on incentive-based performance. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(4), 391–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conty, L., George, N., & Hietanen, J. K. (2016). Watching eyes effects: When others meet the self. Consciousness and Cognition, 45, 184–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conty, L., Gimmig, D., Belletier, C., George, N., & Huguet, P. (2010a). The cost of being watched: Stroop interference increases under concomitant eye contact. Cognition, 115(1), 133–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conty, L., Russo, M., Loehr, V., Hugueville, L., Barbu, S., Huguet, P., et al. (2010b). The mere perception of eye contact increases arousal during a word-spelling task. Social Neuroscience, 5(2), 171–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corston, R., & Colman, A. M. (1996). Gender and social facilitation effects on computer competence and attitudes toward computers. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 14(2), 171–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cottrell, N. B. (1972). Social facilitation. In C. G. McClintock (Ed.), Experimental social psychology. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doenyas, C., Mutluer, T., Genç, E., & Balcı, F. (2019). Error monitoring in decision-making and timing is disrupted in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 12(2), 239–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Double, K. S., & Birney, D. P. (2019). Do confidence ratings prime confidence? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2003, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1553-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2018). Numerical error monitoring. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(4), 1549–1555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2019). Metric error monitoring in the numerical estimates. Consciousness and Cognition, 67, 69–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2020). Monitoring line length reproduction errors. Consciousness and Cognition, 77, 102831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eskenazi, T., Montalan, B., Jacquot, A., Proust, J., Grèzes, J., & Conty, L. (2016). Social influence on metacognitive evaluations: The power of nonverbal cues. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(11), 2233–2247. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1115111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–911. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., & Wellman, H. M. (1977). Metamemory. In R. V. Kail Jr. & J. W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleming, S. M., & Daw, N. D. (2017). Self-evaluation of decision-making: A general Bayesian framework for metacognitive computation. Psychological Review, 124(1), 91–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleming, S. M., & Dolan, R. J. (2012). The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 1338–1349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleming, S. M., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2012a). Metacognition: computation, biology and function. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 1280–1286. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleming, S. M., Huijgen, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2012b). Prefrontal contributions to metacognition in perceptual decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(18), 6117–6125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallucci, M. (2019). GAMLj: General analyses for linear models. [jamovi module]. Retrieved from https://gamlj.github.io/.

  • Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 52–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84(3), 279–325. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, T., & Dajee, K. (2003). Types of task, types of audience, types of actor: Interactions between mere presence and personality type in a simple mathematical task. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(3), 633–639. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00241-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hazem, N., George, N., Baltazar, M., & Conty, L. (2017). I know you can see me: Social attention influences bodily self-awareness. Biological Psychology, 124, 21–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.01.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacquot, A., Eskenazi, T., Sales-Wuillemin, E., Montalan, B., Proust, J., Grèzes, J., et al. (2015). Source unreliability decreases but does not cancel the impact of social information on metacognitive evaluations. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kononowicz, T., Roger, C., & van Wassenhove, V. (2019). Temporal metacognition as the decoding of self-generated brain dynamics. Cerebral Cortex, 29(10), 4366–4380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kononowicz, T. W., & van Wassenhove, V. (2019). Evaluation of Self-generated Behavior: Untangling Metacognitive Readout and Error Detection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(11), 1641–1657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lake, J. I., LaBar, K. S., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Emotional modulation of interval timing and time perception. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 403–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orr, J. M., & Carrasco, M. (2011). The role of the error positivity in the conscious perception of errors. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(16), 5891–5892.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Overgaard, M., & Sandberg, K. (2012). Kinds of access: Different methods for report reveal different kinds of metacognitive access. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Metacognition, 9783642451, 67–85. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-45190-4_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rattat, A. C., & Droit-Volet, S. (2012). What is the best and easiest method of preventing counting in different temporal tasks? Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 67–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riemer, M., Kubik, V., & Wolbers, T. (2019). The effect of feedback on temporal error monitoring and timing behavior. Behavioural Brain Research, 369, 111929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • RStudio Team (2015). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, Inc., Boston, MA URL https://www.rstudio.com/

  • R Core Team (2018). R: A Language and environment for statistical computing. [Computer software]. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/.

  • The jamovi project (2019). jamovi. (Version 0.9) [Computer Software]. Retrieved from https://www.jamovi.org

  • Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition. The American Journal of Psychology, 9(4), 507–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uziel, L. (2007). Individual differences in the social facilitation effect: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(3), 579–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, N., & Summerfield, C. (2012). Metacognition in human decision-making: confidence and error monitoring. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 1310–1321. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.149.3681.269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Alper Mert (undergraduate research assistant at TMDM Lab) for his help with the data collection in Experiment 2.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Fuat Balcı.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Ethical standards

This study has no animal subjects performing any part of the experimental procedures. All procedures that were held in this study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with 1964 Helsinki Declaration. Informed consent form was obtained from all participants prior to the beginning of the experimental procedures.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 22 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Öztel, T., Eskenazi, T. & Balcı, F. Temporal error monitoring with directional error magnitude judgements: a robust phenomenon with no effect of being watched. Psychological Research 85, 2069–2078 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01379-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01379-0

Navigation