Skip to main content
Log in

Phonological encoding is free from orthographic influence: evidence from a picture variant of the phonological Stroop task

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The phonological Stroop task, in which the participant names the color of written distractors, is being used increasingly to study the phonological encoding process in speech production. A brief review of experimental paradigms used to study the phonological encoding process indicated that currently it is not known whether the onset overlap benefit (faster color naming when the distractor shares the onset segment with the color name) in a phonological Stroop task is due to phonology or orthography. The present paper investigated this question using a picture variant of the phonological Stroop task. Participants named a small set of line drawings of animals (e.g., camel) with a pseudoword distractor printed on it. Picture naming was facilitated when the distractor shared the onset segment with the picture name regardless of orthographic overlap (CUST–camel = KUST–camel < NUST–camel). We conclude that the picture variant of the phonological Stroop task is a useful tool to study the phonological encoding process, free of orthographic influence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Starreveld and La Heij’s (2017) paper was in direct opposition to Dell’Aqua, Job, Peressotti and Pascali (2007) who titled their paper “The picture–word interference effect is not a Stroop effect”. In brief, Starreveld and La Heij noted that the results observed by Dell’Aqua et al. and taken as evidence for the dissociation between the two tasks have not been replicated in two later studies, and that the difference is likely to have been due to the methodological differences between the two tasks as they are standardly used. In particular, in the classic Stroop task, but not in the PWI task, only few targets selected from a single semantic category (colors) are used, and the distractors are also drawn from this category. Readers are referred to Starreveld and La Heij (2017) for further detail.

  2. Note that the orthographic (graphemic) overlap is not the same as letter overlap. Specifically, the vowel segment in “seal” is pronounced /i:/ and orthographically represented by the grapheme “ea”, and not “e” (/ɛ/) as in SELP. We deemed SELP, SELM, and SELG (for seal) as acceptable for this reason.

  3. This pattern of finding (CUST = KUST < NUST when naming “camel”) has since been replicated (Kinoshita & Mills, 2020). That study further found no difference between the three distractor conditions (CUST = KUST = NUST) when the response was a manual key press response and did not involve a speech response, consistent with the claim that the effect of onset overlap benefit originates in the phonological encoding process.

  4. In contrast, picture targets used by Lupker (1982) were selected from a children’s coloring book, and the names of some of the pictures (e.g., “fire”) may have been more ambiguous, which may have contributed to the greater role of the name retrieval process.

  5. In this context, we remind the readers that others (e.g., Geng et al., 2014; Shitova et al., 2016) have also noted a substantial reduction in picture naming latency when a small set of pictures is used repeatedly.

References

  • Alario, X. F., Perre, L., Castel, C., & Ziegler, J. C. (2007). The role of orthography in speech production revisited. Cognition, 102, 464–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D. M., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2018). Lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using 'Eigen' and S4. Version 1.1-17.

  • Bowers, J. S., Vigliocco, G., & Haan, R. (1998). Orthographic, phonological, and articulatory contributions to masked letter and word priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1705–1719.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Woollams, A., Kinoshita, S., & Perry, C. (1999). A position-sensitive Stroop effect: Further evidence for a left-to-right component in print-to-speech conversion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 456–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damian, M., & Bowers, J. (2003). Effects of orthography on speech production in a form-preparation paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 119–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dell’Acqua, R., Job, R., Peressotti, F., & Pascali, A. (2007). The picture–word interference effect is not a Stroop effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 717–722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Influence of prime lexicality, frequency, and pronounceability on the masked onset priming effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1813–1837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I., & Davis, C. (1991). The density constraint on form- priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory & Language, 30, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods Instruments and Computers, 35, 116–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geng, J., Schnur, T. T., & Janssen, N. (2014). Relative speed of processing affects interference in Stroop and picture–word interference paradigms: Evidence from the distractor frequency effect. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 1100–1114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press, Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinoshita, S. (2000). The left-to-right nature of the masked onset priming effect in naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 133–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinoshita, S., & Mills, L. (2020). Phonological encoding in the oral but not manual Stroop task: Evidence for the role of a speech production process. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kureta, Y., Fushimi, T., & Tatsumi, I. F. (2006). The functional unit in phonological encoding: Evidence for moraic representation in native Japanese speakers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1102–1119.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2018). lmerTest: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Version 3.0-1. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest. Accessed 02 July 2018.

  • Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupker, S. J. (1982). The role of phonetic and orthographic similarity in picture–word interference. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 349–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, A. S. (1990). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The encoding of successive syllables of a word. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 524–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, A. S. (1991). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 69–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N. (2018). BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs. R package version 0.9.12-4.1. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=BayesFactor. Accessed 02 July 2018.

  • Mousikou, P., Rastle, K., Besner, D., & Coltheart, M. (2015). The locus of serial processing in reading aloud: Orthography-to-phonology computation or speech planning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 41, 1076–1099.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Seaghdha, P. G., Chen, J.-Y., & Chen, T.-M. (2010). Proximate units in word production: Phonological encoding begins with syllables in Mandarin Chinese but with segments in English. Cognition, 115, 282–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Qu, Q., & Damian, M. (2019). Orthographic effects in Mandarin spoken language production. Memory & Cognition, 47, 326–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/. Accessed 02 July 2018.

  • Roelofs, A. (2004). Seriality of phonological encoding in naming objects and reading their names. Memory & Cognition, 32, 212–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roelofs, A. (2006). The influence of spelling on phonological encoding in word reading, object naming, and word generation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 33–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiller, N. O. (2004). The onset effect in word naming. Journal of Memory & Language, 50, 477–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiller, N. O. (2007). Phonology and orthography in reading aloud. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 460–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiller, N. O. (2008). The masked onset priming effect in picture naming. Cognition, 106, 952–962.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shitova, N., Roelofs, A., Schriefers, H., Bastiaansen, M., & Schoffelen, J.-M. (2016). Using brain potentials to functionally localise Stroop-like effects in colour and picture naming: Perceptual encoding versus word planning. PLoS ONE. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0161052.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6(2), 174–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starreveld, P. A., & La Heij, W. (2017). Picture-word interference is a Stroop effect: A theoretical analysis and new empirical findings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24, 721–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verdonschot, R. G., & Kinoshita, S. (2018). Mora or more? The phonological unit of Japanese word production in the Stroop color naming task. Memory & Cognition, 46, 410–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was funded by the Australian Research Council Discovery project scheme (Grant number DP140101199).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sachiko Kinoshita.

Ethics declarations

Open practice statement

The data and output of analyses reported in this paper are available at the Open Science Framework, at the following URL: https://osf.io/c7uyt/.

Conflict of interest

Both authors declare they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Macquarie University Human Research Ethics Committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 39 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kinoshita, S., Verdonschot, R.G. Phonological encoding is free from orthographic influence: evidence from a picture variant of the phonological Stroop task. Psychological Research 85, 1340–1347 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01315-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01315-2

Navigation