Skip to main content
Log in

The influence of threat on perceived spatial distance to out-group members

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A classic example of discriminatory behavior is keeping spatial distance from an out-group member. To explain this social behavior, the literature offers two alternative theoretical options that we label as the “threat hypothesis” and the “shared-experience hypothesis”. The former relies on studies showing that out-group members create a sense of alertness. Consequently, potentially threatening out-group members are represented as spatially close allowing the prevention of costly errors. The latter hypothesis suggests that the observation of out-group members reduces the sharing of somatosensory experiences and, thus, increases the perceived physical distance between oneself and others. In the present paper, we pitted the two hypotheses against each other. In Experiment 1, Caucasian participants expressed multiple implicit “Near/Far” spatial categorization judgments from a Black-African Avatar and a White-Caucasian Avatar located in a 3D environment. Results indicate that the Black-African Avatar was categorized as closer to oneself, as compared with the White-Caucasian Avatar, providing support for “the threat hypothesis”. In Experiment 2, we tested to which degree perceived threat contributes to this categorization bias by manipulating the avatar’s perceived threat orthogonally to group membership. The results indicate that irrespective of group membership, threatening avatars were categorized as being closer to oneself as compared with no threatening avatars. This suggests that provided information about a person and not the mere group membership influences perceived distance to the person.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Lack of coherence means that participants did not express three consecutive “Near” judgments or three consecutive “Far” judgments, respectively in the descending/ascending series. That is, for example a “Near” judgment in a descending series, was followed again by a “Far” judgment, in such case the shift of judgment cannot be considered as being valid, since it is not stable and consistent.

References

  • Ackerman, J. M., Shapiro, J. R., Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Griskevicius, V., et al. (2006). They all look the same to me (unless they’re angry) from out-group homogeneity to out-group heterogeneity. Psychological Science, 17(10), 836–840.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aiello, J. R. (1987). Human spatial behavior. Handbook of Environmental Psychology, 1(1987), 389–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amodio, D. M., & Devine, P. G. (2006). Stereotyping and evaluation in implicit race bias: Evidence for independent constructs and unique effects on behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Avenanti, A., Sirigu, A., & Aglioti, S. M. (2010). Racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with other-race pain. Current Biology, 20(11), 1018–1022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bailenson, J. N., Blascovich, J., Beall, A. C., & Loomis, J. M. (2003). Interpersonal distance in immersive virtual environments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(7), 819–833.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernhardt, B. C., & Singer, T. (2012). The neural basis of empathy. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 1–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchard, R. J., & Blanchard, D. C. (1989). Attack and defense in rodents as ethoexperimental models for the study of emotion. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 13, S3–S14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bourgeois, P., & Hess, U. (2008). The impact of social context on mimicry. Biological Psychology, 77(3), 343–352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buttelmann, D., Zmyj, N., Daum, M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants. Child Development, 84(2), 422–428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coello, Y., Bourgeois, J., & Iachini, T. (2012). Embodied perception of reachable space: how do we manage threatening objects? Cognitive Processing, 13(1), 131–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coello, Y., & Iachini, T. (2015). Embodied perception of objects and people in space. Perceptual and Emotional Embodiment: Foundations of Embodied Cognition, 1, 198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coello, Y. E., & Fischer, M. H. (2016). Foundations of embodied cognition: Perceptual and emotional embodiment. Abingdon: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, S., Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2013). Affective signals of threat increase perceived proximity. Psychological Science, 24(1), 34–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cuddy, A. J., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 61–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Vignemont, F., & Iannetti, G. D. (2015). How many peripersonal spaces? Neuropsychologia, 70, 327–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dotsch, R., & Wigboldus, D. H. (2008). Virtual prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 1194–1198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., & Dunton, B. C. (1997). Categorization by race: The impact of automatic and controlled components of racial prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(5), 451–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferri, F., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Väljamäe, A., Vastano, R., & Costantini, M. (2015). Emotion-inducing approaching sounds shape the boundaries of multisensory peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 70, 468–475.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fini, C., Brass, M., & Committeri, G. (2015). Social scaling of extrapersonal space: target objects are judged as closer when the reference frame is a human agent with available movement potentialities. Cognition, 134, 50–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fini, C., Costantini, M., & Committeri, G. (2014). Sharing space: The presence of other bodies extends the space judged as near. PLoS One, 9(12), e114719.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99(4), 689.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 396–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Genschow, O., Florack, A., & Wänke, M. (2014). Recognition and approach responses toward threatening objects. Social Psychology, 45(2), 86–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genschow, O., & Schindler, S. (2016). The influence of group membership on cross-contextual imitation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1257–1265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goff, P. A., Steele, C. M., & Davies, P. G. (2008). The space between us: Stereotype threat and distance in interracial contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(1), 91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gogel, W. C., & Tietz, J. D. (1977). Eye fixation and attention as modifiers of perceived distance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 45(2), 343–362.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, M. C., Brock, T. C., & Kaufman, G. F. (2004). Understanding media enjoyment: The role of transportation into narrative worlds. Communication Theory, 14(4), 311–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gutsell, J. N., & Inzlicht, M. (2010). Empathy constrained: Prejudice predicts reduced mental simulation of actions during observation of outgroups. Journal of experimental social psychology, 46(5), 841–845.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayduk, L. A. (1983). Personal space: Where we now stand. Psychological Bulletin, 94(2), 293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iachini, T., Coello, Y., Frassinetti, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2014). Body space in social interactions: A comparison of reaching and comfort distance in immersive virtual reality. PLoS One, 9(11), e111511.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Iachini, T., Coello, Y., Frassinetti, F., Senese, V. P., Galante, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2016). Peripersonal and interpersonal space in virtual and real environments: Effects of gender and age. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 45, 154–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iachini, T., Pagliaro, S., & Ruggiero, G. (2015). Near or far? It depends on my impression: Moral information and spatial behavior in virtual interactions. Acta Psychologica, 161, 131–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 887.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2003). Race and gender on the brain: Electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(4), 616.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings (pp. 39–58). NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention.

  • Levy, S. R., West, T. L., Bigler, R. S., Karafantis, D. M., Ramirez, L., & Velilla, E. (2005). Messages about the uniqueness and similarities of people: Impact on US Black and Latino youth. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26(6), 714–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipp, O. V., & Waters, A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background: attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distractors is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion, 7(1), 192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., & Jetten, J. (1994). Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(5), 808.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mooney, K. M., Cohn, E. S., & Swift, M. B. (1992). Physical distance and AIDS: too close for comfort? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22(18), 1442–1452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, R. E. (1928). Human migration and the marginal man. American Journal of Sociology, 33(6), 881–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., et al. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 729–738.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quesque, F., Ruggiero, G., Mouta, S., Santos, J., Iachini, T., & Coello, Y. (2017). Keeping you at arm’s length: modifying peripersonal space influences interpersonal distance. Psychological Research, 81(4), 709–720.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruggiero, G., Frassinetti, F., Coello, Y., Rapuano, M., Di Cola, A. S., & Iachini, T. (2017). The effect of facial expressions on peripersonal and interpersonal spaces. Psychological Research, 81(6):1232–1240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taffou, M., & Viaud-Delmon, I. (2014). Cynophobic fear adaptively extends peri-personal space. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 122.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Pantelidou, G., Rebacz, P., Västfjäll, D., & Tsakiris, M. (2011). I-space: the effects of emotional valence and source of music on interpersonal distance. PLoS One, 6(10), e26083.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., Fiske, S. T., Etcoff, N. L., & Ruderman, A. J. (1978). Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(7), 778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipples, J., Young, A. W., Quinlan, P., Broks, P., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Searching for threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 55(3), 1007–1026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Uzzell, D., & Horne, N. (2006). The influence of biological sex, sexuality and gender role on interpersonal distance. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45(3), 579–597.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valdés-Conroy, B., Román, F. J., Hinojosa, J. A., & Shorkey, S. P. (2012). So far so good: Emotion in the peripersonal/extrapersonal space. PLoS One, 7(11), e49162.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dantzig, S., Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Approach and avoidance as action effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(9), 1298–1306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wist, E. R., & Summons, E. (1976). Spatial and fixation conditions affecting the temporal course of changes in perceived relative distance. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 39(2), 99–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xiao, Y. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2012). See your friends close and your enemies closer: Social identity and identity threat shape the representation of physical distance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(7), 959–972.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xiao, Y. J., Wohl, M. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2016). Proximity under threat: The role of physical distance in intergroup relations. PLoS One, 11(7), e0159792.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yabar, Y., Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Peace, V. (2006). Implicit behavioral mimicry: Investigating the impact of group membership. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30(3), 97–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

Preparation of this paper was supported by Research Programmes G022313N and G073317N of the Research Foundation—Flanders (FWO), Methusalem Grant BOF09/01M00209 of Ghent University, and the Research Fund of KU Leuven (C14/17/047 and GOA/15/003).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chiara Fini.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Authors Chiara Fini, Pieter Verbeke, Sophie Sieber, Agnes Moors, Marcel Brass, Oliver Genschow declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fini, C., Verbeke, P., Sieber, S. et al. The influence of threat on perceived spatial distance to out-group members. Psychological Research 84, 757–764 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1091-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1091-7

Navigation