Skip to main content
Log in

The ontogeny of antipredator behavior: age differences in California ground squirrels (Otospermophilus beecheyi) at multiple stages of rattlesnake encounters

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Newborn offspring of animals often exhibit fully functional innate antipredator behaviors, but they may also require learning or further development to acquire appropriate responses. Experience allows offspring to modify responses to specific threats and also leaves them vulnerable during the learning period. However, antipredator behaviors used at one stage of a predator encounter may compensate for deficiencies at another stage, a phenomenon that may reduce the overall risk of young that are vulnerable at one or more stages. Few studies have examined age differences in the effectiveness of antipredator behaviors across multiple stages of a predator encounter. In this study, we examined age differences in the antipredator behaviors of California ground squirrels (Otospermophilus beecheyi) during the detection, interaction, and attack stages of Pacific rattlesnake (Crotalus oreganus) encounters. Using free-ranging squirrels, we examined the ability to detect free-ranging rattlesnakes, snake-directed behaviors after discovery of a snake, and responses to simulated rattlesnake strikes. We found that age was the most important factor in snake detection, with adults being more likely to detect snakes than pups. We also found that adults performed more tail flagging (a predator-deterrent signal) toward snakes and were more likely to investigate a snake’s refuge when interacting with a hidden snake. In field experiments simulating snake strikes, adults exhibited faster reaction times than pups. Our results show that snake detection improves with age and that pups probably avoid rattlesnakes and minimize time spent in close proximity to them to compensate for their reduced reaction times to strikes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramsky Z, Rosenzweig ML, Subach A (1997) Gerbils under threat of owl predation: isoclines and isodars. Oikos 78:81–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abramsky Z, Rosenzweig ML, Subach A (2002) The costs of apprehensive foraging. Ecology 83:1330–1340

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alberts JR, Cramer CP (1988) Ecology and experience: sources of means and meaning of developmental change. In: Blass EM (ed) Developmental psychobiology and behavioral ecology. Plenum Press, New York, pp 1–39

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Arenz C, Leger D (2000) Antipredator vigilance of juvenile and adult thirteen-lined ground squirrels and the role of nutritional need. Anim Behav 59:535–541

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barbour MA, Clark RW (2012) Ground squirrel tail-flag displays alter both predatory strike and ambush site selection behaviours of rattlesnakes. Proc R Soc Lond B 279:3827–3833

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boellstorff DE, Owings DH (1995) Home range, population structure, and spatial organization of California ground squirrels. J Mammal 76:551–561

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouskila A (1995) Interactions between predation risk and competition: a field study of kangaroo rats and snakes. Ecology 76:165–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broom M, Ruxton GD (2012) Perceptual advertisement by the prey of stalking or ambushing predators. J Theor Biol 315:9–16

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caro TM, FitzGibbon CD (1992) Large carnivores and their prey: die quick and die dead. In: Crawtey MJ (ed) Natural enemies: the population biology of predators. Parasites and diseases. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, pp 117–142

    Google Scholar 

  • Carrier R (1996) Ontogenetic limits on locomotor performance. Physiol Zool 69:467–488

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark RW (2004) Timber rattlesnakes (Crotalus horridus) use chemical cues to select ambush sites. J Chem Ecol 30:607–617

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark RW, Tangco S, Barbour MA (2012) Field video recordings reveal factors influencing predatory strike success of free-ranging rattlesnakes (Crotalus spp.). Anim Behav 84:183–190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook M, Mineka S (1987) Second-order conditioning and overshadowing in the observational conditioning of fear in monkeys. Behav Res Ther 25:349–364

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Correa HKM, Coutinho PEG (1997) Fatal attack of a pit viper, Bothrops jararaca, on an infant buffy-tufted ear marmoset (Callithrix aurita). Primates 38:215–217

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coss RG (1991) Context and animal behavior III: the relationship between early development and evolutionary persistence of ground squirrel antisnake behavior. Ecol Psychol 3:277–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cresswell W (1994) Song as a pursuit-deterrent signal, and its occurrence relative to other anti-predation behaviors of skylark (Alauda arvensis) on attack by merlins (Falco columbarius). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 34:217–223

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Devereux CL (2005) Predator detection and avoidance by starlings under differing scenarios of predation risk. Behav Ecol 17:303–309

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon SM, Baker RL (1988) Effects of size on predation risk, behavioural response to fish, and cost of reduced feeding in larval Ischnura verticalis (Coenagrionidae: Odonata). Oecologia 76:200–205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Embar K, Raveh A, Hoffmann I, Kotler BP (2014) Predator facilitation or interference: a game of vipers and owls. Oecologia 174:1301–1309

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch HS (1949) Study of snake populations in central California. Am Midl Nat 41:513–579

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FitzGibbon CD (1994) The costs and benefits of predator inspection behaviour in Thomson’s gazelles. Anim Behav 34:139–148

    Google Scholar 

  • FitzGibbon CD, Fanshawe JH (1988) Stotting in Thomson’s gazelles: an honest signal of condition. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 23:69–74

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin AS (2004) Social learning about predators: a review and prospectus. Anim Learn Behav 32:131–140

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson MT, Coss RG (1997) Age differences in the response of California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi) to avian and mammalian predators. J Comp Psychol 111:174–84

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson MT, Coss RG (2001) Age differences in arousal and vigilance in California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi). Dev Psychobiol 39:199–206

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasson O (1991) Pursuit-deterrent signals: communication between prey and predator. Trends Ecol Evol 6:325–329

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hawlena D (2009) Colorful tails fade when lizards adopt less risky behaviors. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:205–213

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawlena D, Boochnik R, Abramsky Z, Bouskila A (2006) Blue tail and striped body: why do lizards change their infant costume when growing up? Behav Ecol 17:889–896

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hennessy DF, Owings DH (1977) Snake species discrimination and the role of olfactory cues in the snake-directed behavior of the California ground squirrel. Behaviour 65:115–123

  • Hennessy F, Owings H (1988) Rattlesnakes create a context for localizing their search for potential prey. Ethology 77:317–329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hersek MJ (1990) Behavior of predator and prey in a highly coevolved system: northern Pacific rattlesnakes and California ground squirrels. Thesis, University of California, Davis

  • Hersek MJ, Owings DH (1993) Tail flagging by adult California ground squirrels: a tonic signal that serves different functions for males and females. Anim Behav 46:129–138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hersek M, Owings DH (1994) Tail flagging by young California ground squirrels, Spermophilus beecheyi: age-specific participation in a tonic communicative system. Anim Behav 48:803–811

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes CM (1994) Social learning in animals: categories and mechanisms. Biol Rev 69:207–231

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hileman KS, Brodie ED Jr (1994) Survival strategies of the salamander Desmognathus ochrophaeus: interaction of predator-avoidance and anti-predator mechanisms. Anim Behav 47:1–6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollén LI, Manser MB (2006) Ontogeny of alarm call responses in meerkats, Suricata suricatta: the roles of age, sex and nearby conspecifics. Anim Behav 72:1345–1353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins GR, Gall BG, Brodie ED Jr (2011) Ontogenetic shift in efficacy of antipredator mechanisms in a top aquatic predator, Anax junius (Odonata: Aeshnidae). Ethology 117:1093–1100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones KA, Krebs JR, Whittingham MJ (2009) Heavier birds react faster to predators: individual differences in the detection of stalking and ambush predators. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63:1319–1329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehmeier S, Schloegl C, Scheiber IBR, Weiß BM (2011) Early development of gaze following into distant space in juvenile Greylag geese (Anser anser). Anim Cogn 14:477–485

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kotler BP, Blaustein L, Brown JS (1992) Predator facilitation: the combined effect of snakes and owls on the foraging behavior of gerbils. Ann Zool Fenn 29:199–206

    Google Scholar 

  • Kotler BP, Brown JS, Dall SRX, Gresser S, Ganey D, Bouskila A (2002) Foraging games between gerbils and their predators: temporal dynamics of resource depletion and apprehension in gerbils. Evol Ecol Res 4:495–518

    Google Scholar 

  • Kotler BP, Brown JS, Slotow RH, Goodfriend WL, Strauss M (1993) The influence of snakes on the foraging behavior of gerbils. Oikos 67:309–316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landová E, Jančúchová-Lásková J, Musilová V, Kadochová S, Frynta D (2013) Ontogenetic switch between alternative antipredatory strategies in the leopard gecko (Eublepharis macularius): defensive threat versus escape. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67:1113–1122

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL (2002) Putting predators back into behavioral predator–prey interactions. Trends Ecol Evol 17:70–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:619–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lind J, Cresswell W (2005) Determining the fitness consequences of antipredation behavior. Behav Ecol 16:945–956

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mateo JM (1996a) Early auditory experience and the ontogeny of alarm-call discrimination in Belding’s ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi). J Comp Psychol 110:115–124

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mateo JM (1996b) The development of alarm-call response behaviour in free-living juvenile Belding’s ground squirrels. Anim Behav 52:489–505

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCourt ME, Jacobs GH (1983) Effects of photic environment on the development of spectral response properties of optic nerve fibers in the ground squirrel. Exp Brain Res 49:443–452

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Medill SA, Renard A, Larivière S (2011) Ontogeny of antipredator behaviour in striped skunks (Mephitis mephitis). Ethol Ecol Evol 23:41–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nonacs P, Blumstein D (2010) Predation risk and behavioral life history. In: Westneat DF, Fox CW (eds) Evolutionary behavioral ecology. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 207–221

    Google Scholar 

  • Owings DH, Coss RG (1977) Snake mobbing by California ground squirrels: adaptive variation and ontogeny. Behaviour 62:50–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owings DH, Coss RG (2008) Hunting California ground squirrels: constraints and opportunities for northern Pacific rattlesnakes. In: Hayes WK, Beaman KR, Cardwell MD, Bush SP (eds) Biology of the rattlesnakes. Loma Linda University Press, Loma Linda, pp 155–167

    Google Scholar 

  • Owings DH, Loughry WJ (1985) Variation in snake-elicited jump-yipping by black-tailed prairie dogs: ontogeny and snake-specificity. Z Tierpsychol 70:177–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Platzen D, Magrath RD (2005) Adaptive differences in response to two types of parental alarm call in altricial nestlings. Proc R Soc Lond B 272:1101–1106

  • Poran NS, Coss RG (1990) Development of antisnake defenses in California ground squirrels (Spermopilus beecheyi): I. Behavioral and immunological relationships. Behaviour 112:222–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poran NS, Coss RG, Benjamini E (1987) Resistance of California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi) to the venom of the northern Pacific rattlesnake (Crotalus viridis oreganus): a study of adaptive variation. Toxicon 25:767–777

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Putman BJ, Clark RW (2015) The fear of unseen predators: ground squirrel tail flagging in the absence of snakes signals vigilance. Behav Ecol 26:185–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramakrishnan U, Coss RG, Schank J, Dharawat A, Kim S (2005) Snake species discrimination by wild bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). Ethology 111:337–356

  • Ramakrishnan U, Coss RG (2000) Age differences in the responses to adult and juvenile alarm calls by bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). Ethology 106:131–144

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reinert HK, Cundall D (1982) An improved surgical implantation method for radio-tracking snakes. Copeia 1982:702–705

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reinert HK, MacGregor GA, Esch M, Bushar LM, Zappalorti RT (2011) Foraging ecology of timber rattlesnakes, Crotalus horridus. Copeia 2011:430–442

  • Sheets LP, Dean KF, Reiter LW (1988) Ontogeny of the acoustic startle response and sensitization to background noise in the rat. Behav Neurosci 102:706–713

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sordahl TA (1988) The American avocet (Recurvirostra americana) as a paradigm for adult automimicry. Evol Ecol 2:189–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swaisgood RR, Rowe MP, Owings DH (2003) Antipredator responses of California ground squirrels to rattlesnakes and rattling sounds: the roles of sex, reproductive parity, and offspring age in assessment and decision-making rules. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:22–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tromborg CT, Coss RG (2015) Isolation rearing reveals latent antisnake behavior in California ground squirrels (Otospermophilus becheeyi) searching for predatory threats. Anim Cogn. doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0853-5

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trulio LA (1996) The functional significance of infanticide in a population of California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38:97–103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wasko DK, Bonilla F, Sasa M (2014) Behavioral responses to snake cues by three species of neotropical rodents. J Zool 292:142–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitfield DP (2003) Redshank Tringa totanus flocking behaviour, distance from cover and vulnerability to sparrowhawk Accipiter nisus predation. J Avian Biol 34:163–169

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whittingham MJ, Butler SJ, Quinn JL, Cresswell W (2004) The effect of limited visibility on vigilance behaviour and speed of predator detection: implications for the conservation of granivorous passerines. Oikos 106:377–385

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiedenmayer CP (2009) Plasticity of defensive behavior and fear in early development. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33:432–441

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Funding for this research was provided by San Diego State University, the National Geographic Society Waitts Grant (W17-08 to RWC), the National Science Foundation (DBI-0951010 to RWC), the Mildred E. Mathias Graduate Student Research Grant (to B JP), and the Animal Behavior Society Student Research Grant (to BJP). For assistance with fieldwork, we thank Matthew Barbour, Caesar Rahman, Emily Mastrelli, Jessica Fort, Zachary Cava, Michelle Goh, Elinor Israel, Emily Stulik, Hossein Ayazi, Eric Schroder, Cleo Grieve, Sean Tangco, Kenneth Huang, Curt Barnes, Darren Fraser, Rey Ayon, Annie Maguire, Matthew Strimas-Mackey, Gretchen Anderson, Tara Easter, Jessica Tingle, Erynn Rebol, Mark Herr, Trevor Darragh, Lauren Kong, Susan Anthony, Mike Hogan, Joseph Chase, and Jenny Schefski. For assistance with reviewing and quantifying video recordings, we thank Omar Singleton, Sky Jacobson, Ashley Benson, Armando Magana, Ivana Bui, Rebecca Ehrlich, Sarfaraz Serang, Jenny Schefski, Jaime Lane McKenzie, and Alexis Leon. We thank Michael Hamilton for general field support and for assistance with erecting the wireless internet network at the BORR field site, and members of the 2013 Clark Lab and two anonymous reviewers for comments improving the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical standards

All methods were approved by the San Diego State University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (APF 10-09-025C).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Breanna J. Putman.

Additional information

Communicated by P. B. Banks

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Putman, B.J., Coss, R.G. & Clark, R.W. The ontogeny of antipredator behavior: age differences in California ground squirrels (Otospermophilus beecheyi) at multiple stages of rattlesnake encounters. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 69, 1447–1457 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-015-1957-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-015-1957-2

Keywords

Navigation