Skip to main content
Log in

Accuracy of spatial localization depending on head posture in a perturbed gravitoinertial force field

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Spatial orientation is crucial when subjects have to accurately reach memorized visual targets. In previous studies modified gravitoinertial force fields were used to affect the accuracy of pointing movements in complete darkness without visual feedback of the moving limb. Target mislocalization was put forward as one hypothesis to explain this decrease in accuracy of pointing movements. The aim of this study was to test this hypothesis by determining the accuracy of spatial localization of memorized visual targets in a perturbed gravitoinertial force field. As head orientation is involved in localization tasks and carrying relevant sensory systems (visual, vestibular and neck muscle proprioceptive), we also tested the effect of head posture on the accuracy of localization. Subjects (n=10) were seated off-axis on a rotating platform (120° s−1) in complete darkness with the head fixed (head-fixed session) or free to move (head-free session). They were required to report verbally the egocentric spatial localization of visual memorized targets. They gave the perceived target location in direction (i.e. left or right) and in amplitude (in centimeters) relative to the direction they thought to be straight ahead. Results showed that the accuracy of visual localization decreased when subjects were exposed to inertial forces. Moreover, subjects localized the memorized visual targets more to the right than their actual position, that was in the direction of the inertial forces. With further analysis, it appeared that this shift of localization was concomitant with a shift of the visual straight ahead (VSA) in the opposite direction. Thus, the modified gravitoinertial force field led to a modification in the orientation of the egocentric reference frame. Furthermore, this shift of localization increased when the head was free to move while the head was tilted in roll toward the center of rotation of the platform and turned in yaw in the same direction. It is concluded that the orientation of the egocentric reference frame was influenced by the gravitoinertial vector.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The Coriolis force is related to the mass of a moving segment in a rotating frame, its velocity in the rotating frame and the angular velocity of the rotating frame of reference. Centrifugal force is related to the product of the square angular velocity, the mass of the subject in rotation, and the distance of the subject relative to the center of rotation.

  2. Equation 1 is the Probit function used to evaluate the VSA. Variables: P i =probability for the ith target to be perceived to the right, i=target number, j=trial number; Parameters: C0=target for P=0.5, n=power of the effect

References

  • Biguer B, Prablanc C, Jeannerod M (1984) The contribution of coordinated eye and head movements in hand pointing accuracy. Exp Brain Res 55:462–469

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blouin J, Vercher J-L, Gauthier GM, Paillard J, Bard C, Lamarre Y (1995) Perception of passive whole-body rotations in the absence of neck and body proprioception. J Neurophysiol 74:2216–2219

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock O, Arnold KE, Cheung BSK (1996a) Performance of a simple aiming task in hypergravity: I. overall accuracy. Aviat Space Environ Med 67:127–132

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bock O, Arnold KE, Cheung BSK (1996b) Performance of a simple aiming task in hypergravity: II. Detailed response characteristics. Aviat Space Environ Med 67:133–138

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdin C, Gauthier GM, Blouin J, Vercher J-L (2001) Visual feedback of the moving arm allows complete adaptation of pointing movements to centrifugal and coriolis forces. Neurosci Lett 301:25–28

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdin C, Prieur J-M, Sarès F, Gauthier GM, Menu J-P, Montmayeur A (2002) Head position control and target localization performance in changing gravito-inertial field. RTO HFM Symposium on “Spatial disorientation in military vehicles: causes, consequences and cures”. RTO-MP-086.

  • Bringoux L, Schmerber S, Nougier V, Dumas G, Barraud PA, Raphel C (2002) Perception of slow pitch and roll body tilts in bilateral labyrinthine-defective subjects. Neuropsychology 40:367–372

    Google Scholar 

  • Bringoux L, Tamura K, Faldon M, Gresty MA, Bronstein AM (2004) Influence of whole-body pitch tilt and kinesthetic cues on the perceived gravity-referenced eye level. Exp Brain Res 155:385–392

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clement G, Moore ST, Raphan T, Cohen B (2001) Perception of tilt (somatogravic illusion) in response to sustained acceleration during space flight. Exp Brain Res 138:410–418

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coello Y, Orliaget JP, Parblanc C (1996) Pointing movement in an artificial perturbing inertial field: a prospective paradigm for motor control study. Neuropsychology 34:879–892

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen MM (1977) Disorienting effects of aircraft catapult launching: III. cockpit displays and piloting performance. Aviat Space Environ Med 48:797–804

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen MM, Stoper AE, Welch RB, DeRoshia CW (2001) Effects of gravitational and optical stimulation on the perception of target elevation. Percept Psychophys 63:29–35

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cusack R, Carlyon RP, Robertson IH (2001) Auditory midline and spatial discrimination in patient with unilateral neglect. Cortex 37:706–709

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darling WG, Miller GF (1995) Perception of arm orientation in three-dimensional space. Exp Brain Res 102:495–502

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget M, Pélisson D, Rossetti Y, Prablanc C (1998) From eye to hand: planning goal-directed movements. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 22:761–788

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dizio P, Lackner JR (1995) Motor adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm. J Neurophysiol 74:1787–1792

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farne A, Ponti F, Ladavas E (1998) In search of biased egocentric reference frames in neglect. Neuropsychology 36:611–623.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisk J, Lackner JR, Dizio P (1993) Gravitoinertial force level influences arm movement control. J Neurophysiol 69:504–510

    Google Scholar 

  • Fookson OV, Smetanin BN, Berkinblit MB, Adamovich SA, Feldman G, Poizner H (1994) Azimuth errors in pointing to remembered targets under extreme head rotations. Neuroreport 5:885–888

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg FE, Fernandez C (1984) The vestibular system. In: Smith I (ed) Handbook of physiology. The nervous system, vol 3. New York, Academic Press, pp 977–1021

  • Howard IP (1986) The preception of posture, self motion, and the visual vertical. In: Boff K, Kaufman L, Thomas J (eds) Handbook of perception and human performance. Wiley, New York, pp 1–62

  • Howard IP, Templeton WB (1966) Human spatial orientation. Wiley, London

  • Jeannerod M (1988) The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements. Oxford University Press, Oxford

  • Karn KS, Moller P, Hayhoe MM (1997) Reference frames in saccadic targeting. Exp Brain Res 115:267–282

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karnath HO, Sievering D, Fetter M (1994) The interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation to subjective ‘‘straight ahead’’ orientation in man. Exp Brain Res 101:140–146

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner JR, Dizio P (1998) Gravitoinertial force background levels affects adaptation to coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements. J Neurophysiol 80:546–553

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner JR, Dizio P (1994) Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbation of arm trajectory. J Neurophysiol 72:299–313

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacquaniti F (1997) Frames of reference in sensorimotor coordination. In: Boller F, Grafman J (eds) Handbook of neuropsychology. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 27–64

  • Lewald J, Ehrenstein WH (2000) Visual and proprioceptive shifts in perceived egocentric direction induced by eye-position. Vision Res 40:539–547

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maurer C, Kimming H, Trefzer A, Mergner T (1997) Visual object localization through vestibular and neck inputs. 1: Localization with respect to space and relative to head and trunk mid-sagital planes. J Vest Res 7:119–135

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McDougall HG, Curthoys IS, Betts GA, Burgess AM, Halmagyi GM (1999) Human ocular conterrolling during roll-tilt centrifugation. Ann NY Acad Sci 28:173–180

    Google Scholar 

  • Mergner T, Nasios G, Maurer C, Becker W (2001) Visual object localization in space: interaction of retinal, eye position, vestibular and neck proprioceptive information. Exp Brain Res 141:33–51

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mittelstaedt H (1983) A new solution to the problem of the subjective vertical. Naturwissenschaften 70:272–281

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mittelstaedt H (1988) The information processing structure of the subjective vertical. A cybernetic bridge between its psychophysics and its neurobiology. In: Marko H, Hauske G, Struppler A (eds) Processing structures for perception and action. VCH, Weinheim, pp 217–263

  • Moore ST, Clément G, Raphan T, Cohen B (2001) Ocular counterrolling induced by centrifugation during orbital. Exp Brain Res 137:323–335

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paillard J (1991) Motor space and representational framing of space. In: Paillard J (ed) Brain and Space. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 163–182

  • Sarès F, Prieur J-M, Bourdin C, Gauthier GM, Blouin J, Vercher J-L (2002) Peripheral visual information influence head posture during gravitoinertial changes. Curr Pychol Cog 21:455–477

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarès F, Bourdin C, Prieur J-M, Vercher J-L, Gauthier GM (2004) Head positioning control in a gravitoinertial field and in normal gravity. J Vestib Res 14: 321–333

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Tillery SIH, Flanders M (1990) Transformation from head- to shoulder-centered representation of target direction in arm movements. J Cogn Neurosci 2:32–43

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodman PD, Griffin MJ (1997) Effect of direction of head movement on motion sickness caused by coriolis stimulation. Aviat Space Environ Med 62:93–98

    Google Scholar 

  • Young LR (1984) Perception of the body in space: mechanisms. In: Geiger SR (ed) Handbook of physiology III/1. Bethesda, pp 1023–1066

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grants from Dassault aviation. Special thanks to Citroën Competition for the bucket seat, to Alain Donneaud for technical expertise, and to Frank Buloup for programming.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J.-M. Prieur.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Prieur, JM., Bourdin, C., Vercher, JL. et al. Accuracy of spatial localization depending on head posture in a perturbed gravitoinertial force field. Exp Brain Res 161, 432–440 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-2087-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-2087-7

Keywords

Navigation