Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The effects of acute abstinence from smoking and performance-based rewards on performance monitoring

  • Original Investigation
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Rationale

Abstinence from smoking disrupts performance in multiple cognitive domains, and such cognitive effects may serve to maintain smoking behavior. Rather than having specific effects on a narrow domain of processing, abstinence may disrupt more general cognitive control processes and/or motivation.

Objectives

The present study tested the prediction that overnight abstinence from smoking would disrupt a general performance monitoring system indexed via the error-related negativity (ERN). A secondary aim was to determine the extent to which performance-based monetary rewards improved the ERN among smokers and whether the effect of the reward was diminished during abstinence.

Methods

The ERN was assessed during a flanker task among 25 heavy, non-treatment-seeking smokers both when smoking as usual and after overnight abstinence; reward and no-reward trial blocks occurred within each session.

Results

As predicted, mean ERN amplitude was reduced during abstinence. The ERN was enhanced by reward; this effect did not vary with smoking abstinence.

Conclusion

This study provides novel data which suggest that acute abstinence from smoking disrupts a neurophysiological index of a general performance monitoring system that is involved in a range of cognitive functions. The ERN may be a useful complement to narrow-band cognitive studies of abstinence and interventions designed to target cognition in addiction. Because the ERN was concurrently sensitive to abstinence and performance-based incentives, it may be particularly useful for examining the interplay of cognition and motivation in smoking and smoking cessation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Self-report data were missing from one participant during the abstinence visit due to computer failure.

  2. We had hoped to examine the impact of abstinence on the feedback-related negativity (FRN). Although all participants met the recommended minimum of six trials per condition for the ERN (Olvet and Hajcak 2009), only 56 % met the recommended minimum of 20 trials per condition for the FRN (Marco-Pallares et al. 2010). Given the small subsample (n = 14), the FRN was never analyzed.

  3. Grand average waveforms for all valid correct and incorrect trials were collapsed across congruency. This approach was favored for two reasons. First, we sought to maximize the number of error trials included in waveform computations for the purpose of stability. Second, participants committed very few errors on congruent trials during no reward blocks (abstinent: M = 3.9, SD = 4.2, range = 0–15; smoking: M = 3.3, SD = 3.2, range = 0–11) as well as reward blocks (abstinent: M = 2.3, SD = 3.4, range = 0–14; smoking: M = 1.9, SD = 2.3, range = 0–7).

  4. Supplemental RT analyses evaluated fast error trials (rather than all error trials) and fast correct trials. Mean reaction time did not differ by smoke condition [F(1,5) = 3.28, p = 0.13]. RTs were faster for error trials [F(1,5) = 19.51, p = 0.007, d = 1.80], but did not interact with smoke condition [F(1,5) = 2.59, p = 0.17]. A main effect of congruency was present and indicated that RTs were significantly slower on incongruent trials [F(1,5) = 15.93, p = 0.01, d = 0.99]. The congruency × accuracy [F(1,5) = 2.58, p = 0.17] and smoke condition × congruency × accuracy interactions [F(1,5) = 2.72, p = 0.16] were not significant. Reward had no effect on RTs [F(1,5) = 0.01, p = 0.93]. Similarly, all interactions involving reward were not significant (all F < 0.73, all p > 0.43).

References

  • Baker TB, Brandon TH, Chassin L (2004) Motivational influences on cigarette smoking. Annu Rev Psychol 55:463–491

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartholow BD, Henry EA, Lust SA, Saults JS, Wood PK (2012) Alcohol effects on performance monitoring and adjustment: affect modulation and impairment of evaluative cognitive control. J Abnorm Psychol 121(1):173–186

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boksem MA, Tops M, Wester AE, Meijman TF, Lorist MM (2006) Error-related ERP components and individual differences in punishment and reward sensitivity. Brain Res 1101(1):92–101

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick MM, Noll D, Cohen JD (1998) Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science 280(5364):747–749

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu PH, Deldin PJ (2007) Neural evidence for enhanced error detection in major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 164(4):608–616

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen S, Lichtenstein E, Prochaska JO, Rossi JS, Gritz ER, Carr CR et al (1989) Debunking myths about self-quitting. Evidence from 10 prospective studies of persons who attempt to quit smoking by themselves. Am Psychol 44(11):1355–1365

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cox LS, Tiffany ST, Christen AG (2001) Evaluation of the brief Questionnaire of Smoking Urges (QSU-Brief) in laboratory and clinical settings. Nicotine Tob Res 3(1):7–16

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Davies PL, Segalowitz SJ, Gavin WJ (2004) Development of response-monitoring ERPs in 7- to 25-year-olds. Dev Neuropsychol 25(3):355–376

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins L, Powell JH, West R, Powell J, Pickering A (2006) A double-blind placebo controlled experimental study of nicotine: I—effects on incentive motivation. Psychopharmacology 189(3):355–367

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • de Bruijn ER, Hulstijn W, Verkes RJ, Ruigt GS, Sabbe BG (2004) Drug-induced stimulation and suppression of action monitoring in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology 177:151–160

    Google Scholar 

  • de Bruijn ER, Sabbe BG, Hulstijn W, Ruigt GS, Verkes RJ (2006) Effects of antipsychotic and antidepressant drugs on action monitoring in healthy volunteers. Brain Research 1105:122–129

    Google Scholar 

  • de Wit H (2009) Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addict Biol 14(1):22–31

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Di Chiara G, Imperato A (1988) Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 85:5274–5278

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Domier CP, Monterosso JR, Brody AL, Simon SL, Mendrek A, Olmstead R et al (2007) Effects of cigarette smoking and abstinence on Stroop task performance. Psychopharmacology 195(1):1–9

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Easdon C, Izenberg A, Armilio ML, Yu H, Alain C (2005) Alcohol consumption impairs stimulus- and error-related processing during a Go/No-Go Task. Cogn Brain Res 25(3):873–883

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Epping-Jordan MP, Watkins SS, Koob GF, Markou A (1998) Dramatic decreases in brain reward function during nicotine withdrawal. Nature 393(6680):76–79

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW (1974) Effects of noise letters upon identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept Psychophys 16(1):143–149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein M, Hohnsbein J, Hoormann J, Blanke L (1991) Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. 2. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 78(6):447–455

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Franken IH, van Strien JW, Franzek EJ, van de Wetering BJ (2007) Error-processing deficits in patients with cocaine dependence. Biol Psychol 75(1):45–51

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franken IH, van Strien JW, Kuijpers I (2009) Evidence for a deficit in the salience attribution to errors in smokers. Drug and Alcohol Depend 106(2–3):181–185

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehring WJ, Goss B, Coles MGH, Meyer DE, Donchin E (1993) A neural system for error-detection and compensation. Psychol Sci 4(6):385–390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak G (2012) What we’ve learned from mistakes: insights from error-related brain activity. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 21(2):101–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak G, Moser JS, Yeung N, Simons RF (2005) On the ERN and the significance of errors. Psychophysiology 42(2):151–160

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall JR, Bernat EM, Patrick CJ (2007) Externalizing psychopathology and the error-related negativity. Psychol Sci 18(4):326–333

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerstrom KO (1991) The Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence—a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict 86(9):1119–1127

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heishman SJ, Kleykamp BA, Singleton EG (2010) Meta-analysis of the acute effects of nicotine and smoking on human performance. Psychopharmacology 210(4):453–469

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hendricks PS, Ditre JW, Drobes DJ, Brandon TH (2006) The early time course of smoking withdrawal effects. Psychopharmacology 187(3):385–396

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd CB, Coles MG (2002) The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 109(4):679–709

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz-Kraus T, Breznitz Z (2009) Can the error detection mechanism benefit from training the working memory? A comparison between dyslexics and controls—an ERP study. PLoS One 4(9):e7141

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes JR (2007) Effects of abstinence from tobacco: valid symptoms and time course. Nicotine Tob Res 9(3):315–327

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes JR, Hatsukami D (1986) Signs and symptoms of tobacco withdrawal. Arch Gen Psychiatry 43(3):289–294

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobsen LK, Krystal JH, Mencl WE, Westerveld M, Frost SJ, Pugh KR (2005) Effects of smoking and smoking abstinence on cognition in adolescent tobacco smokers. Biol Psychiatry 57(1):56–66

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johannes S, Wieringa BM, Nager W, Dengler R, Munte TF (2001) Oxazepam alters action monitoring. Psychopharmacology 155:100–106

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kalamboka N, Remington B, Glautier S (2009) Nicotine withdrawal and reward responsivity in a card-sorting task. Psychopharmacology 204(1):155–163

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kassel JD (1997) Smoking and attention: a review and reformulation of the stimulus-filter hypothesis. Clin Psychol Rev 17(5):451–478

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen WL, Fulton EK (2008) Cigarette abstinence impairs memory and metacognition despite administration of 2 mg nicotine gum. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 16(6):521–531

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koob GE, Le Moal M (2008) Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annu Rev Psychol 59:29–53

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer UM, Cunillera T, Camara E, Marco-Pallares J, Cucurell D, Nager W et al (2007) The impact of catechol-O-methyltransferase and dopamine D4 receptor genotypes on neurophysiological markers of performance monitoring. J Neurosci 27(51):14190–14198

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Larson MJ, Clayson PE (2011) The relationship between cognitive performance and electrophysiological indices of performance monitoring. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 11(2):159–171

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lerman C, Perkins KA, Gould TJ (eds) (2009) Phenotypes and endophenotypes: foundations for genetic studies of nicotine use and dependence. National Cancer Institute, Bethesda, MD

    Google Scholar 

  • Leventhal AM, Waters AJ, Moolchan ET, Heishman SJ, Pickworth WB (2010) A quantitative analysis of subjective, cognitive, and physiological manifestations of the acute tobacco abstinence syndrome. Addict Behav 35(12):1120–1130

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liotti M, Pliszka SR, Perez R, Kothmann D, Woldorff MG (2005) Abnormal brain activity related to performance monitoring and error detection in children with ADHD. Cortex 41(3):377–388

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luijten M, van Meel CS, Franken IH (2011) Diminished error processing in smokers during smoking cue exposure. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 97(3):514–520

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marco-Pallares J, Cucurell D, Munte TF, Strien N, Rodriguez-Fornells A (2010) On the number of trials needed for a stable feedback-related negativity. Psychophysiology 48(6):852–860

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClernon FJ, Kollins SH, Lutz AM, Fitzgerald DP, Murray DW, Redman C et al (2008) Effects of smoking abstinence on adult smokers with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: results of a preliminary study. Psychopharmacology 197(1):95–105

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mendrek A, Monterosso J, Simon SL, Jarvik M, Brody A, Olmstead R et al (2006) Working memory in cigarette smokers: comparison to non-smokers and effects of abstinence. Addict Behav 31(5):833–844

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merritt PS, Cobb AR, Cook GI (2012) Sex differences in the cognitive effects of tobacco abstinence: a pilot study. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 20:258–263

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller AE, Watson JM, Strayer DL (2012) Individual differences in working memory capacity predict action monitoring and the error-related negativity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 38(3):757–763

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olvet DM, Hajcak GJ (2009) The stability of error-related brain activity with increasing trials. Psychophysiology 46:957–961

    Google Scholar 

  • Olvet DM, Hatchwell E, Hajcak G (2010) Lack of association between the 5-HTTLPR and the error-related negativity (ERN). Biol Psychol 85(3):504–508

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pailing PE, Segalowitz SJ (2004) The error-related negativity as a state and trait measure: motivation, personality, and ERPs in response to errors. Psychophysiology 41(1):84–95

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson F, Jepson C, Loughead J, Perkins K, Strasser AA, Siegel S et al (2010) Working memory deficts predict short-term smoking resumption following brief abstinence. Drug and Alcohol Depend 106:61–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potts GF (2011) Impact of reward and punishment motivation on behavior monitoring as indexed by the error-related negativity. Int J Psychophysiol 81(3):324–331

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Potts GF, Martin LE, Kamp SM, Donchin E (2010) Neural response to action and reward prediction errors: comparing the error-related negativity to behavioral errors and the feedback-related negativity to reward prediction violations. Psychophysiology 48(2):218–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell JH, Al-Adawi S, Morgan J, Greenwood RJ (1996) Motivational deficits after brain injury: effects of bromocriptine in 11 patients. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 60(4):416–421

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Powell J, Dawkins L, Davis RE (2002) Smoking, reward responsiveness, and response inhibition: tests of an incentive motivational model. Biol Psychiatry 51(2):151–163

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Powell JH, Pickering AD, Dawkins L, West R, Powell JF (2004) Cognitive and psychological correlates of smoking abstinence, and predictors of successful cessation. Addict Behav 29(7):1407–1426

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Powell J, Dawkins L, West R, Powell J, Pickering A (2010) Relapse to smoking during unaided cessation: clinical, cognitive and motivational predictors. Psychopharmacology 212:537–549

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Riba J, Rodriguez-Fornells A, Munte TF, Barbanoj MJ (2005a) A neurophysiological study of the detrimental effects of alprazolam on human action monitoring. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 25:554–565

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Riba J, Rodriguez-Fornells A, Morte A, Munte TF, Barbanoj MJ (2005b) Noradrenergic stimulation enhances human action monitoring. J Neurosci 25:4370–4374

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ridderinkhof KR, de Vlugt Y, Bramlage A, Spaan M, Elton M, Snel J et al (2002) Alcohol consumption impairs detection of performance errors in mediofrontal cortex. Science 298:2209–2211

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving—an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev 18(3):247–291

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2000) The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: an incentive-sensitization view. Addiction 95(8):S91–S117

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2003) Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol 54:25–53

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2008) The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philos Trans R Soc Biol Sci 363(1507):3137–3146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rukstalis M, Jepson C, Patterson F, Lerman C (2005) Increases in hyperatice-impulsive symptoms predict relapse among smokers in nicotine replacement therapy. J Subst Abus Treat 28:297–304

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Santesso DL, Segalowitz SJ, Schmidt LA (2005) ERP correlates of error monitoring in 10-year olds are related to socialization. Biol Psychol 70(2):79–87

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schellekens AFA, de Bruijn ERA, van Lankveld CAA, Hulstijn W, Buitelaar JK, de Jong CAJ et al (2010) Alcohol dependence and anxiety increase error-related brain activity. Addiction 105(11):1928–1934

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schlam TR, Japuntich SJ, Piper ME, Gloria R, Baker TB, Curtin JJ (2011) Cognitive conflict following appetitive versus negative cues and smoking cessation failure. Psychopharmacology 214:603–616

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Semlitsch HV, Anderer P, Schuster P, Presslich O (1986) A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology 23(6):695–703

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shiels K, Hawk LW Jr (2010) Self-regulation in ADHD: the role of error processing. Clin Psychol Rev 30(8):951–961

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffman S, Paty JA, Gnys M, Elash C, Kassel JD (1995) Nicotine withdrawal in chippers and regular smokers: subjective and cognitive effects. Heal Psychol 14(4):301–309

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sofuoglu M (2010) Cognitive enhancement as a pharmacotherapy target for stimulant addiction. Addiction 105(1):38–48

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sokhadze E, Stewart C, Hollifield M, Tasman A (2008) Event-related potential study of executive dysfunctions in a speeded reaction task in cocaine addiction. J Neurother 12(4):185–204

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spronk D, Dumont GJH, Verkes RJ, de Bruijn ERA (2011) Acute effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol on performance monitoring in healthy volunteers. Front Behav Neurosci 5(59):1–11

    Google Scholar 

  • Themanson JR, Rosen PJ, Pontifex MB, Hillman CH, McAuley E (2012) Alterations in error-related brain activity and post-error behavior over time. Brain Cogn 80:257–265

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tieges Z, Ridderinkhof KR, Snel J, Kok A (2004) Caffeine strengthens action monitoring: evidence from the error-related negativity. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 21:87–93

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Welsch SK, Smith SS, Wetter DW, Jorenby DE, Fiore MC, Baker TB (1999) Development and validation of the Wisconsin Smoking Withdrawal Scale. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 7(4):354–361

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zirnheld PJ, Carroll CA, Kieffaber PD, O’Donnell BF, Shekhar A, Hetrick WP (2004) Haloperidol impairs learning and error-related negativity in humans. J Cognitive Neurosci 16(6):1098–1112

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors appreciate the assistance of Rebecca Ashare with recruitment and Nicholas Albino with data collection. We are grateful to Stephen Tiffany for his feedback and suggestions throughout the project and on prior versions of the manuscript. We also thank the reviewers for their suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Larry W. Hawk Jr..

Additional information

Author note

This research was completed in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the doctoral degree of NJS under the supervision of LWH. KSR is now at the Kennedy Krieger Institute, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD. Portions of this research were presented at 2011 Annual Meeting of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Schlienz, N.J., Hawk, L.W. & Rosch, K.S. The effects of acute abstinence from smoking and performance-based rewards on performance monitoring. Psychopharmacology 229, 701–711 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-013-3131-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-013-3131-8

Keywords

Navigation