Skip to main content
Log in

Examining The Reinforcing Properties of Making Sense: A Preliminary Investigation

  • Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Acceptance and commitment therapy asserts that in clinical problems such as rumination and depression, making sense continues despite accompanying aversive consequences, because sense-making is reinforcing, particularly when it leads to experiential avoidance. The following series of experiments aimed to provide preliminary empirical evidence for this hypothesis by comparing college students’ preferences for a solvable laboratory task with response-contingent reinforcement to a formally similar but unsolvable task, on which equal amounts of reinforcement were presented independent of performance. When asked to choose, participants reliably preferred solvable or neutral tasks over unsolvable tasks according to self-report as well as concurrent and forced-choice behavioral procedures. These studies also aimed to investigate whether individuals preferred the solvable condition because it was positively reinforcing or because avoiding the unsolvable condition was negatively reinforcing. Results are consistent with the theory that making sense may continue despite adverse consequences because of experiential avoidance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ADDIS, M. E., & CARPENTER, K. M. (1999). Why, why, why? Reason-giving and rumination as predictors of response to activation- and insight-oriented treatment rationales. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 881–894.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ADDIS, M. E., & JACOBSON, N. S. (1996). Reasons for depression and the process and outcome of cognitive-behavioral psychotherapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1417–1424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • AFFLECK, G., TENNEN, H., CROOG, S., & LEVINE, S. (1987). Causal attribution, perceived benefits, and morbidity after a heart attack: An 8-year study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 29–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ANTONOVSKY, A. (1979). Health, stress, and coping. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • AVERILL, J. R. (1973). Personal control over aversive stimuli and its relationship to stress. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 286–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BECK, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • BECK, A. T., RUSH, A. J., SHAW, B. F., & EMERY, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • BIRD, A., & REESE, E. (2006). Emotional reminiscing and the development of an autobiographical self. Developmental Psychology, 42, 613–626.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BORKOVEC, T. D., ROBINSON, E., PRUZINSKY, T., & DEPREE, J. A. (1983). Preliminary exploration of worry: Some characteristics and processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 9–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BOWER, J. E., KEMENY, E., TAYLOR, S. E., & FAHEY, J. L. (1998). Cognitive processing, discovery of meaning, Cd4 decline, and Aids-related mortality among bereaved HIV-seropositive men. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 979–986.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BROCKNER, J., & HULTON, A. (1978). How to reverse the vicious cycle of low self-esteem: The importance of attentional focus. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 564–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BRUNER, A., & REVUSKY, S. (1961). Collateral behavior in humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 349–350.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BURGER, J. M., & ARKIN, R. M. (1980). Prediction, control and learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 482–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BUSS, A. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety. San Fransisco, CA: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • CALHOUN, L., & TEDESCHI, R. (2000). Early posttraumatic interventions: Facilitating possibilities for growth. Posttraumatic stress intervention: Challenges, issues, and perspectives (pp. 135–152). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • CARVER, C. S., & SCHEIER, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control-therapy approach to human behavior. Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • CARVER, C. S., & SCHEIER, M. F. (1986). Functional and dysfunctional responses to anxiety: The interaction between expectancies and self-focused attention. In R. Schwartzer (Ed.), Self-related cognitions in anxiety and motivation (pp. 111–141). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • CATANIA, C. A. (1992). Reinforcement schedules. In J. Sanns (Ed.), Learning (3rd ed., pp. 157–185). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • CHAPMAN, I., & CHAPMAN, J. (1967). Genesis of popular but erroneous psychodiagnostic observations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 193–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DUVAL, S., & WICKLUND, R. (1972). A theory of objective self awareness. Oxford, England: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ELLIS, A. (1962). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy. Secaucus, NJ: Citadel.

    Google Scholar 

  • ELLIS, A. (1990). Special features of rational-emotive therapy. In W. Dryden & R. DiGuisseppe (Eds.), A primer on rational-emotive therapy (pp. 79–93). Champaign, IL: Research Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • GERGEN, K. J., & GERGEN, M. M. (1988). Narrative and the self as relationship. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 17–56). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GERWIRTZ, J. L., & STENGLE, K. G. (1968). Learning of generalized imitation as the basis for identification. Psychological Review, 5, 374–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GLASS, D. C., & SINGER, J. E. (1972). Urban noise: Experiments on noise and social stressors. New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • GOLDING, S. L., & RORER, L. G. (1972). “Illusory correlation” and the learning of clinical judgment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 80, 249–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HARRISON, R. J., & GREEN, G. (1990). Development of conditional and equivalence relations without differential consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 225–237.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., BARNES-HOLMES, D., & ROCHE, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., LUOMA, J., BOND, F., MASUDA, A., & LILLIS, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes, and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., STROSAHL, K. D., & WILSON, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior therapy. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., STROSAHL, K. D., WILSON, K. G., BISSETT, R. T., PISTORELLO, J., TOARMINO, D., … MCCURRY, S. M. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model. The Psychological Record, 54, 553–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., & WILSON, K. G. (1994). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Altering the verbal support for experiential avoidance. The Behavior Analyst, 17, 289–303.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HOLYOAK, K. J., & SIMON, D. (1999). Bidirectional reasoning in decision making by constraint satisfaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 128, 3–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HULL, J. (1981). A self-awareness model of the causes and effects of alcohol consumption. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 586–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • INGRAM, R. E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: Review and a conceptual model. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 156–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • JANOFF-BULMAN, R., & FRIEZE, I. (1983). A theoretical perspective for understanding reactions to victimization. Journal of Social Issues, 39, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • KELLEY, H. H. (1972). Attribution in social interaction. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the cause of behavior (pp. 1–26). Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • KELLEY, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LEWINSOHN, P. M., HOBERMAN, H., TERI, L., & HAUTZINGER, M. (1985). An integrative theory of depression. In S. Reiss & R. Bootzin (Eds.), Theoretical issues in behavior therapy (pp. 331–359). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LYUBOMIRSKY, S., & NOLEN-HOEKSEMA, S. (1993). Self-perpetuating properties of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 339–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MARTIN, L. L., & TESSER, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Ruminative thoughts (pp. 1–47). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • MAXWELL, S. E., & DELANEY, H. D. (2004). Designing experiments and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • MCADAMS, D. P., REYNOLDS, J., LEWIS, M., PATTEN, A., & BOWMAN, P. J. (2001). When bad things turn good and good things turn bad: Sequences of redemption and contamination in life narrative, and their relation to psychosocial adaptation in midlife adults and in students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 472–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MENDOLA, R., TENNEN, H., AFFLECK, G., MCCANN, L., & FITZGERALD, R. (1990). Appraisal and adaption among women with impaired fertility. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 79–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MINEKA, S., & HENDERSON, R. W. (1985). Controllability and predictability in acquired motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 495–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MUSSON, R., & ALLOY, L. (1988). Depression and self-directed attention. Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 193–220). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • NATALE, M., DAHLBERG, C., & JAFFE, J. (1978). The relationship of defensive language behavior in patient monologues to the course of psychoanalysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 352–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • NOLEN-HOEKSEMA, S. (1991). Response to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episode. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • PALS, J. (2006). Constructing the “springboard effect”: Causal connections, selfmaking, and growth within the life story. In D. P. McAdams, R. Josselson, & A. Lieblich (Eds.), Identity and story: Creating self in narrative (pp. 175–199). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • PAPAGEORGIOU, C., & SIEGLE, G. (2003). Rumination and depression: Advances in theory and research. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 243–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PAPAGEORGIOU, C., & WELLS, A. (2001). Metacognitive beliefs about rumination in recurrent major depression. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 8, 160–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PAPAGEORGIOU, C., & WELLS, A. (2003). An empirical test of a clinical metacognitive model of rumination and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 261–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PARK, R. J., GOODYER, I. M., & TEASDALE, J. D. (2004). Effects of rumination and distraction on mood and overgeneral autobiographical memory in adolescent major depressive disorder and controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 996–1006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • PENNEBAKER, J. W., & SEAGAL, J. D. (1999). Forming a story: The health benefits of narrative. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 1243–1254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • PETERSON, C., & SELIGMAN, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression. Psychological Review, 91, 347–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • PYSZCZYNSKI, T., & GREENBERG, J. (1986). Evidence for a depressive self-focusing style. Journal of Research in Personality, 20, 95–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PYSZCZYNSKI, T., & GREENBERG, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and the depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reactive depression. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ROTHBAUM, F., WEISZ, J., & SNYDER, S. (1982). Changing the world and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 5–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SARASON, I. G. (1975). Anxiety and self-preoocupation. In I. G. Saranson & C. D. Spielberger (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (pp. 129–151). New York, NY: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • SARASON, I. G. (1986). Test anxiety, worry, and cognitive interference. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-related cognitions in anxiety and motivation (pp. 19–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHACHTER, S., & SINGER, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1936). The verbal summator and a method for the study of latent speech. Journal of Psychology, 2, 71–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York, NY: The Macmillan Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • SOSNOWSKI, T. (1983). Sources of uncertainty and psychophysiological state: Changes under conditions of noxious stimulus anticipation. Biological Psychology, 17, 297–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SOSNOWSKI, T. (1988). Patterns of skin conductance and heart rate changes under anticipatory stress conditions. Journal of Psychophysiology, 2, 231–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • STARR, B. J., & KATLIN, E. S. (1969). The clinical as an aberrant actuary: Illusory correlation and incomplete sentences blank. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 74, 670–675.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SUNDBERG, M., & MICHAEL, J. (2001). The benefits of Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior for children with autism. Behavior Modification, 25, 698–723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • VAUGHAN, M. E., & MICHAEL, J. (1982). Automatic reinforcement: An important but ignored concept. Behaviorism, 10, 217–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • WARD, W. C., & JENKINS, H. M. (1965). The display of information and the judgment of contingency. Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 19, 231–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WATKINS, E. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163–206.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WINE, J. D. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 92–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WINE, J. D. (1982). Evaluation anxiety: A cognitive-attentional construct. In H. W. Krohne & L. Laux (Eds.), Achievement, stress, and anxiety (pp. 207–219). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • WRAY, A. M., & DOUGHER, M. J. (2009, May). Reinforcing functions of making sense using socially relevant stimuli. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis International Convention, Phoenix, AZ.

    Google Scholar 

  • WRAY, A. M., DOUGHER, M. J., & BULLARD, L. (2008, May). Examining the reinforcing effects of making sense. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis International Convention, Chicago, IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • WRAY, A. M., FREUND, R. A., & DOUGHER, M. J. (2009). A behavior analytic account of cognitive bias in clinical populations. The Behavior Analyst, 31, 29–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alisha M. Wray.

Additional information

This research was partially supported by a grant funded by the Student Resources Allocation Committee of the Graduate and Professional Student Association at the University of New Mexico.

We would like to thank Lauren Bullard, Seamus Gentz, Beth Leitman, and Dustin Truitt for their assistance in data collection and Josh Tybur for his assistance with data analyses.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wray, A.M., Dougher, M.J., Hamilton, D.A. et al. Examining The Reinforcing Properties of Making Sense: A Preliminary Investigation. Psychol Rec 62, 599–622 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395823

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395823

Keyword

Navigation