Skip to main content
Log in

Motivating operations affect stimulus control: A largely overlooked phenomenon in discrimination learning

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Several recent studies have explored what Michael (e.g., 1982) termed the value-altering effect and the behavior-altering effect of motivating operations. One aspect of the behavior-altering effect that has garnered no recent attention involves changes in stimulus control produced by motivating operations. To call attention to this aspect of the behavior-altering effect, we herein review 11 studies that are concerned with the influence of varying levels of food or water deprivation on stimulus generalization. These studies suggest that motivating operations influence stimulus control (a) by changing the evocative strength of not just an established discriminative stimulus, but also of stimuli that are physically similar to it; (b) by changing the range of stimuli that evoke the operant in question; and (c) by exerting these effects in a graded fashion. These findings are potentially of conceptual and applied significance, and it appears that further research examining how motivating operations alter stimulus control, including some studies suggested herein, is warranted.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Brown, J. S. (1942). The generalization of approach responses as a function of stimulus intensity and strength of motivation. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 33, 209–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, F. C. (1958). The effect of deprivation and frequency of reinforcement on variable-interval responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 221–228.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Coate, W. B. (1964). Effects of deprivation on postdiscrimination stimulus generalization in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 57, 134–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gaiardi, M., Bartoletti, M., Bacchi, A., Gubellini, C., & Babbini, M. (1987). Increased sensitivity to the stimulus properties of morphine in food deprived rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior, 26, 719–723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, N., & Kalish, H. (1956). Discriminability and stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 79–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Healey, A. F. (1965). Compound stimuli, drive strength, and primary stimulus generalization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 536–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hearst, E. (1962). Concurrent generalization gradients for food-controlled and shock-controlled behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 19–31.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Horner, R. H., Day, H. M., & Day, J. R. (1997). Using neutralizing routines to reduce problem behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 601–604.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Iwata, B. A., Smith, R. G., & Michael, J. (2000). Current research on the influence of establishing operations on behavior in applied settings. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 411–418.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, W. O., Pascal, G. R., & Walker, R. W. (1958). Deprivation and generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 274–277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalish, H. L., & Haber, A. (1965). Prediction of discrimination from generalization following variations in deprivation level. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 60, 125–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, F. S., & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). Principles of psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Killeen, P. R., & Hall, S. S. (2001). The principal components of response strength. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75, 111–134.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Laraway, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A. (2003). Motivating operations and terms to describe them: Some further refinements. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 407–414.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Li, M., Garner, H. R., Wessinger, W. D., & McMillan, D. E. (1995). Effects of food deprivation and satiation on sensitivity to the discriminative-stimulus effects of pentobarbital in pigeons and morphine in rats. Behavioural Pharmacology, 6, 724–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1981). The foundations of ethology. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Massey, B. W., & McMillan, D. E. (1987). Effects of body weight on discriminative-stimulus control by phencyclidine in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 233–239.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative and motivational functions of stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149–155.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1993). Establishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 191–206.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, J. R., & Grice, G. R. (1965). Stimulus generalization as a function of drive level, and the relation between two measures of response strength. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 357–362.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, R. W. (1971). Evidence of interaction between deprivation effects and stimulus control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 16, 95–104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sundberg, M. L., & Michael, J. (2001). The benefits of Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior for children with autism. Behavior Modification, 25, 698–724.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. R., & King, K. A. (1959). Stimulus generalization as a function of level of motivation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 323–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1948). Social releasers and the experimental method required for their study. Wilson Bulletin, 60, 6–51.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alan Poling.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lotfizadeh, A.D., Edwards, T.L., Redner, R. et al. Motivating operations affect stimulus control: A largely overlooked phenomenon in discrimination learning. BEHAV ANALYST 35, 89–100 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392268

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392268

Key words

Navigation