Skip to main content
Log in

Behavioral Economics

A Tutorial for Behavior Analysts in Practice

  • Article
  • Published:
Behavior Analysis in Practice Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In recent years, behavioral economics has gained much attention in psychology and public policy. Despite increased interest and continued basic experimental studies, the application of behavioral economics to therapeutic settings remains relatively sparse. Using examples from both basic and applied studies, we provide an overview of the principles comprising behavioral economic perspectives and discuss implications for behavior analysts in practice. A call for further translational research is provided.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ainslie, G. (1974). Impulse control in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21, 485–489.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ariely, D. (2008). Predictably irrational: The hidden forces that shape our decisions. New York, NY: Harper-Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ariely, D. (2010). The upside of irrationality: The unexpected benefits of defying logic at work and at home. New York, NY: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., Edwards, G., Laneri, M., Fletcher, K., & Metevia, L. (2001). Executive functioning, temporal discounting, and sense of time in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 541–556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., Green, L., & Vuchinich, R. E. (1995). Behavioral economics. Journal of the experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 257–262.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T., & Gatchalian, K. M. (2011). The behavioral economics and neuroeconomics of reinforcer pathologies: Implications for etiology and treatment of addiction. Current Psychiatry Reports, 13, 406–415.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., Marsch, L. A., & Carroll, M. E. (2000). Deconstructing relative reinforcing efficacy and situating the measures of pharmacological reinforcement with behavioraleconomics: A theoretical proposal. Psychopharmacology, 153, 44–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borrero, J. C., Francisco, M. T., Haberlin, A. T., Ross, N. A., & Sran, S. K. (2007). A unit price evaluation of severe problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 463–474.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Broussard, C., & Northup, J. (1997). The use of functional analysis to develop peer interventions for disruptive classroom behavior. School Psychology Quarterly, 12, 65–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burns, M. K. (2011). School psychology research: Combining ecological theory and prevention science. School Psychology Review, 40, 132–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C. (1999). Behavioral economics: Reunifying psychology and economics. Proceedings of the National Academies of Science, 96, 10575–10577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C. F., Loewensein, G., & Rabin, M. (Eds.). (2004). Advances in behavioral economics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press & Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, G. B. (1996). Temporal discounting and utility for health and money. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 771–791.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collier, G., Johnson, D. F., & Morgan, C. (1992). The magnitude-of-reinforcement function in closed and open economies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 81–89.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Critchfield, T., & Reed, D. D. (2004). Conduits of translation in behavior-science bridge research. In E. Ribes Iñesta & J. E. Burgos (Eds.), Theory, basic and applied research, and technological applications in behavior science: Conceptual and methodological issues (pp. 45–84). Guadalajara, Mexico: University of Guadalajara Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. R., Horner, M. J., & Guercio, J. (2003). Self-control and the preference for delayed reinforcement: An example in brain injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 371–374.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Francisco, M. T., Madden, G. J., & Borrero, J. (2009). Behavioral economics: Principles, procedures, and utility for applied behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst Today, 10, 277–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., & Freed, D. E. (1993). The substitutability of reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 141–158.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., Myerson, J., & Ostaszewski, P. (1999). Discounting of delayed rewards across the life span: Age differences in individual discounting functions. Behavioural Processes, 46, 89–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, R. R., Brady, J. V., & Bradford, L. D. (1979). Predicting the abuse liability of drugs and animal drug self-administration procedures: Psychomotor stimulants and hallucinogens. In T. Thompson & P. B. Dews (Eds.), Advances in behavioral pharmacology (Vol. 2, pp. 163–208). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosch, J., & Neuringer, A. (1981). Self-control in pigeons under the Mischel paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 3–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Grunwald, M. (2009, April 2). How Obama is using the science of change. Time Magazine, April. Retrieved from http://www.time.com.

  • Hackenberg, T. D. (2009). Token reinforcement: A review and analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 91, 257–286.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hillier, B. (1991). The macroeconomic debate: Models of the closed and open economy. New York, NY: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hursh, S. R. (1980). Economic concepts for the analysis of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34, 219–238.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hursh, S. R. (1984). Behavioral economics. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 435–452.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hursh, S. R. (2000). Behavioral economic concepts and methods for studying health behavior. In W. K. Bickel & R. E. Vuchinich (Eds.), Reframing health behavior change with behavioral economics (pp. 27–62). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hursh, S. R., Madden, G. J., Spiga, R., DeLeon, I. G., & Francisco, M. T. (2013). The translational utility of behavioral economics: The experimental analysis of consumption and choice. In Madden, G. J., Dube, W. V., Hackenberg, T. D., Hanley, G. P., & Lattal, K. A. (Eds.), APA handbook of behavior analysis: Vol. 2. Translating principles into practice (pp. 191–224). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hursh, S. R., Raslear, T. G., Shurtleff, D., Bauman, R., & Simmons, L. (1988). A cost-benefit analysis of demand for food. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 419–440.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hursh, S. R., & Roma, P. G. (2013). Behavioral economics and empirical public policy. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99, 98–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2002). Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77, 129–146.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2006). Replacing relative reinforcing efficacy with behavioral economic demand curves. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85, 73–93.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B. & Rachlin, H. (2006). Social discounting. Psychological Science, 17, 283–286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Slovic, A. & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. & Bootzin, R. B. (1972). The token economy: An evaluative review. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 243–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, K. N., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1995). Preference reversals due to myopic discounting of delayed reward. Psychological Science, 6, 83–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kodak, T., Lerman, D. C., & Call, N. (2007). Evaluating the influence of postsession reinforcement on choice of reinforcers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 515–527.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1991). The transfer of contextual control over equivalence classes through equivalence classes: A possible model of social stereotyping. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 505–518.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LaFiette, M. H., & Fantino, E. (1989). Responding on concurrent-chains schedules in open and closed economies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 329–342.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J. (1691). Some considerations of the consequences of the lowering of interest, and raising the value of money: In a letter sent to a member of parliament. London: Awnsham and John Churchill. Retrieved from http://books.google.com

    Google Scholar 

  • Mace, F. C., & Critchfield, T. S. (2010). Translational research in behavior analysis: Historical traditions and imperative for the future. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, 293–312.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Madden, G. J. (2000). A behavioral economics primer. In W. K. Bickel & R. E. Vuchinich (Eds.), Reframing health behavior change with behavioral economics (pp. 3–26). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madden, G. J., Begotka, A. M., Raiff, B. R., & Kastern, L. L. (2003). Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11, 139–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Madden, G. J., & Bickel, W. K. (2010). Impulsivity: The behavioral and neurological science of discounting. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Martens, B. K., & Kelly, S. Q. (1993). A behavioral analysis of effective teaching. School Psychology Quarterly, 8, 10–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior: Vol. V. The effect of delay and intervening events on reinforcement value (pp. 55–73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neef, N. A., & Lutz, M. N. (2001). Assessment of variables affecting choice and application to classroom interventions. School Psychology Quarterly, 16, 239–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neef, N. A., Shade, D., & Miller, M. S. (1994). Assessing influential dimensions of reinforcers on choice in students with serious emotional disturbance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 575–583.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Persky, J. (1995). Retrospectives: The ethology of homo economicus. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 9, 221–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poling, A. (2010). Progressive-ratio schedule and applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 347–349.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H., Green, L., Kagel, J. H., & Battalio, R. C. (1976). Economic demand theory and psychological studies of choice. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 10, pp. 129–154). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H., Raineri, A., & Cross, D. (1991). Subjective probability and delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 233–244.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, D. D. (2008). The translation of basic behavioral research to school psychology: A citation analysis. The Behavior Analyst Today, 9, 143–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, D. D., DiGennaro Reed, F. D., Chok, J., & Brozyna, G. A. (2011). The “tyranny of choice”: Choice overload as a possible instance of effort discounting. The Psychological Record, 61, 547–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, D. D., & Martens, B. K. (2011). Temporal discounting predicts student responsiveness to exchange delays in a classroom token system. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 1–18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roane, H. (2008). On the applied use of progressive-ratio schedules of reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 155–161.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roane, H. S., Call, N. A., & Falcomata, T. S. (2005). A preliminary analysis of adaptive responding under open and closed economies. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 335–348.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roane, H. S., Falcomata, T. S., & Fisher, W. W. (2007). Applying the behavioral economics principle of unit price to DRO schedule thinning. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 529–534.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roane, H. S., Lerman, D. C., & Vorndran, C. M. (2001). Assessing reinforcers under progressive schedule requirements. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 145–167.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Salvy, S. J., Nitecki, L. A., & Epstein, L. H. (2009). Do social activities substitute for food in youth? Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 38, 205–212.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Samuelson, P. A., & Nordhaus, W. D. (1985). Economics. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheres, A., Dijkstra, M., Ainslie, E., Balkan, J., Reynolds, B., Sonuga-Barke, E., & Castellanos, F. X. (2006). Temporal and probabilistic discounting of rewards in children and adolescents: Effects of age and ADHD symptoms. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2092–2103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schouwenburg, H. C., & Groenewoud, J. T. (2001). Study motivation under social temptation: Effects of trait procrastination. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 229–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schweitzer, J. B., & Sulzer-Azaroff, B. (1988). Self-control: Teaching tolerance for delay in impulsive children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 173–186.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sheridan, S. M., & Gutkin, T. B. (2000). The ecology of school psychology: Examining and changing our paradigm for the 21st century. School Psychology Review, 29, 485–502.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheridan, S. M., Kratochwill, T. R., & Bergan, J. R. (1996). Conjoint behavioral consultation: A procedural manual. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York, NY: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Thaler, R. H. (1990). Anomalies: Preference reversals. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 4, 201–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Derek D. Reed.

Additional information

The authors thank the various clinicians and behavior analysts with whom they have worked with over the years that prompted the writing of this tutorial, as well as Scott Wiggins and Dave Jarmolowicz for their assistance to the authors during the course of manuscript preparation. Finally, they acknowledge the role of their Applied Behavioral Science (ABSC) 509 students for persistently asking for examples of how basic behavioral science translates to practice. The examples derived from these conversations and discussions have been integrated throughout the tutorial.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Reed, D.D., Niileksela, C.R. & Kaplan, B.A. Behavioral Economics. Behav Analysis Practice 6, 34–54 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391790

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391790

Keywords

Navigation