Skip to main content
Log in

Emotional distress and the perceived interference of menstruation

  • Published:
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The relation of perceived interference of menstruation on expected behaviors to emotional distress was examined. It was predicted that the perceived interference of menstruation would be more predictive of emotional distress associated with menstruation than either menstrual or premenstrual pain. Participants completed measures of menstrual pain and visual analogue scales of emotional distress (anger, frustration, depression, anxiety, fear). Measures of perceived ability to tolerate the pain, the perceived interference of the pain, and attitudes toward menstruation were also collected. Results indicated that perceived interference was the strongest predictor of emotional distress secondary to menstrual pain. Ratings of menstrual and premenstrual pain were also significant predictors of emotional distress. Results supported the major hypothesis of the study and provide evidence that the appraisal of interference imposed by a particular condition or Stressor may be an important factor in stress and coping processes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • AuBuchon, P. G., & Calhoun, K. S. (1985). Menstrual cycle symptomatology: The role of social expectancy and experimental demand characteristics,Psychosomatic Medicine, 47, 35–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Averill, J. R. (1982).Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression.American Psychologist, 8, 1145–1160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Azrin, N. H., Hutchinson, R. R., & Hake, D. (1966). Extinction-induced aggression.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 119–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unified theory of behavioral change.Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1973).Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation.Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, J., Ruble, D. N., & Clarke, A. (1977). College women's attitudes and expectations concerning menstrual-related changes.Psychosomatic Medicine, 39, 288–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J., & Ruble, D. N. (1980). The Menstrual Attitude Questionnaire.Psychosomatic Medicine, 42, 503–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chernovetz, M. E., Jones, W. H., & Hansson, R. O. (1979). Predictability, attentional focus, sex role orientation, and menstrual-related stress.Psychosomatic Medicine, 41, 383–391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cox, D. J. (1983). Menstrual symptoms in college students: A controlled study.Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 6, 335–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dollard, J., Doob, L., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R. (1939).Frustration and aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, T. (1992). Problem solving appraisal, oral contraceptive use, and menstrual pain.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 286–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, T., & Harkins, S. W. (1991). Psychosocial concomitants of persistent pain among persons with spinal cord injuries.NeuroRehabilitation, 1(4), 9–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample.Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21, 219–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). Coping as a mediator of emotion.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 466–475.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Dunkel-Schetter, C., DeLongis, A., & Gruen, R. J. (1986). Dynamics of a stressful encounter: Cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 992–1003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Gruen, R. J., & DeLongis, A. (1986). Appraisal, coping, health status, and psychological symptoms.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 571–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fradkin, B., & Firestone, P. (1986). Premenstrual tension, expectancy, and mother-child relations.Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 9, 245–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. G., & Elliott, T. (1987). Life stress and psychologic adjustment among persons with spinal cord injury.Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 68, 344–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. G., Umlauf, R., Wonderlich, S. Askanazi, G., Buckelew, S., & Elliott, T. (1987). Differences in coping styles among persons with spinal cord injury: A cluster analytic approach.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 727–731.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallant, S. J., & Hamilton, J. A. (1988). On a premenstrual psychiatric diagnosis: What's in a name?Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 19, 271–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallant, S. J., Hamilton, J. A., Popiel, D. A., Morokoff, P., & Chakraboty, P. (1991). Daily moods and symptoms: Effects of awareness of study focus, gender, menstrual-cycle phase, and day of the week.Health Psychology, 10, 180–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, J. A., & Gallant, S. J. (1990). Problematic aspects of diagnosing premenstrual phase dysphoria: Recommendations for psychological research and practice.Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 21, 60–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harkins, S. W., Price, D. D., & Braith, J. (1989). Effects of extraversion and neuroticism on experimental pain, clinical pain, and illness behavior.Pain, 36, 209–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koff, E., Rierdan, J., & Stubbs, M. (1990). Conceptions and misconceptions of the menstrual cycle.Women and Health, 16(3/4), 119–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984).Stress, appraisal, and coping. Springer: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leventhal, H., & Mosbach, P. (1983). The perceptual-motor theory of emotion. In J. Cacioppo & R. Petty (Eds.),Social psychophysiology (pp. 353–388). Guilford Press: London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logue, C. M., & Moos, R. H. (1986). Perimenstrual symptoms: Prevalence and risk factors.Psychosomatic Medicine, 48(6), 388–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacFarlane, J., Martin, C. L., & Williams, T. M. (1988). Mood fluctuations: Women versus men and menstrual versus other cycles.Psychology of Women Quarterly, 12, 201–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moos, R. (1968). The development of a Menstrual Distress Questionnaire.Psychosomatic Medicine, 30, 853–867.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moos, R. (1984).Menstrual Distress Questionnaire manual. Palo Alto, CA: Department of Psychiatry, Stanford University and Veterans Administration.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paige, K. E. (1973). Women learn to sing the menstrual blues.Psychology Today, 7, 41–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parlee, M. B. (1973). The premenstrual syndrome.Psychological Bulletin, 80, 454–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parlee, M. B. (1974). Stereotypic beliefs about menstruation: A methodological note on the Moos Distress Questionnaire and some new data.Psychosomatic Medicine, 36, 229–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parlee, M. B. (1982). Changes in mood and activation levels during the menstrual cycle in experimentally naive subjects.Psychology of Women Quarterly, 12, 119–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, D. D., & Barrell, J. (1984). Some general laws of human emotion: Interrelationships between intensities of desire, expectation, and emotional feeling.Journal of Personality, 52, 389–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, D. D., Barrell, J. F., & Barrell, J. J. (1985). A quantitative-experiential analysis of human emotions.Motivation and Emotion, 9, 19–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Revenson, T. A., & Felton, B. J. (1989). Disability and coping as predictors of psychological adjustment to rheumatoid arthritis.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 344–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodin, J. (1976). Menstruation, reattribution, and competence.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 345–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Menstrual symptoms: A social cognition analysis.Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 2, 171–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. A., Clancy, S., & Vitaliano, P. P. (1987). Relationships of stress, appraisal and coping to chronic low back pain.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25, 281–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wade, J. B., Price, D., Hamer, R., Schwartz, S., & Hart, R. (1990). An emotional component analysis of chronic pain.Pain, 40, 303–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion.Psychological Review, 92, 548–573.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Elliott, T.R., Harkins, S.W. Emotional distress and the perceived interference of menstruation. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 14, 293–306 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00962634

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00962634

Key words

Navigation