Skip to main content
Log in

Cross-sectional and longitudinal observations of activity level and sustained attention in a normative sample

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined developmental changes in playroom observations of activity level and sustained attention among 47 elementary school-age boys. The boys were observed at two different times, approximately 2 years apart. The playroom observations consisted of two 15-minute periods, a relatively unstructured freeplay period and a structured restricted academic period. In general, both cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses suggested developmental improvements in the observed behaviors. Both gross and fine motor activity decreased with increasing age, while the time spent on task increased. Consistent with the selective attention literature, there appeared to be a dramatic increase in the boys' ability of focus attention at around age 11. Finally, most of the behaviors in the restricted academic period showed significant stability over time, whereas few of the free-play behaviors did so.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abikoff, H., Gittelman, R., & Klein, D. F. (1980). Classroom observation code for hyper-active children: A replication of validity.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 555–565.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abikoff, H., Gittelman-Klein, R., & Klein, D. F. (1977). Validation of a classroom observation code for hyperactive children.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 772–783.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1978a). The child behavior profile: I. Boys aged 6–11.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 478–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achenback, T. M. (1978b).Research in developmental psychology: Concepts, strategies methods. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1982).Developmental psychopathology (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1981). Behavioral problems and competencies reported by parents of normal and disturbed children aged 4 through 16.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46, (Serial No. 168).

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1980).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed). Washington, D.C.: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. T. (1982). A developmental analysis of visual and auditory sustained attention and reflection-impulsivity in hyperactive and normal children.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 15, 614–618.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M., Block, J. N., & Block, J. (1980). Preschool activity level: Personality correlates and developmental implications.Child Development, 51, 401–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, S. B. (1983). Developmental perspectives in child psychopathology. In T. H. Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.),Handbook of child psychopathology (pp. 13–40). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Copeland, A. P. (1979). Types of private speech produced by hyperactive and nonhyperactive boys.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 7, 169–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, V. I., & Peters, K. G. (1979). Toward a clearer definition of the attentional deficit of hyperactive children. In G. Hale & M. Lewis (Eds.),Attention and cognitive development (pp. 173–247). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furman, W. (1979). Promoting appropriate social behavior: Developmental implications for treatment. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.),Advances in clinical child psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 1–40). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gale, A., & Lynn, R. (1972). A developmental study of attention.British Journal of Educational Psychology, 42, 260–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gersten, J. C., Langner, T. S., Eisenberg, J. G., Simcha-Fagan, O., & McCarthy, E. D. (1976). Stability and change in types of behavioral disturbance of children and adolescents.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 4, 111–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glow, R. A., Glow, P. H., & Rump, E. E. (1982). The stability of child behavior disorders: A one year test-retest study of Adelaide versions of the Conners teacher and parent rating scales.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 10, 33–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goyette, C. H., Conners, C. K., & Ulrich, R. F. (1978). Normative data on revised Conners parent and teacher rating scales.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6, 221–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagen, J. W., & Hale, G. H. (1973). The development of attention in children. In A. Pick (Ed.),Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 117–140). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halverson, C. F., & Waldrop, M. F. (1976). Relations between preschool activity and aspects of intellectual and social behavior at age 71/2.Developmental Psychology, 12, 107–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A. B., & Redlich, F. C. (1958).Social class and mental illness. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehman, E. B. (1972). Selective strategies in children's attention to task-relevant information.Child Development, 43, 197–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, F. (1980). The development of sustained attention (vigilance) and inhibition in children: Some normative data.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 21, 77–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loney, J., & Milich, R. (1982). Hyperactivity, inattention, and aggression in clinical practice. In M. Wolraich & D. Routh (Eds.),Advances in developmental and behavioral pediatrics (Vol. 2, pp. 113–147). Greenwich, Connecticut: J.AI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackworth, J. F. (1976). Development of attention. In V. Hamilton & M. Vernon (Eds.),The development of cognitive processes (pp. 111–152). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattes, J. A. (1980). The role of frontal lobe dysfunction in childhood hyperkinesis.Comprehensive Psychiatry, 21, 358–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCall, R. B. (1977). Challenges to a science of developmental psychology.Child Development, 48, 333–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milich, R., Loney, J., & Landau, S. (1982). The independent dimensions of hyperactivity and aggression: A validation with playroom observation data.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 183–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milich, R., Loney, J., & Whitten, P. (1983, August).Two-year stability and validity of playroom observations of hyperactivity. Paper presented at the annual meetng of the American Psychological Association, Anaheim, California.

  • Miller, P. H., & Weiss, M. G. (1981). Children's attention allocation, understanding of attention, and performance on the incidental learning task.Child Development, 52, 1183–1190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. C. (1973). Research strategies and measurement methods for investigating human development. In J. R., Nesselroade & H. W. Reese (Eds.),Life-span developmental psychology: Methodological issues (pp. 87–109). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelham, W. E. (in press). The measurement of attention in attention deficit disorders.Clinical Psychology Review.

  • Perelle, I. B. (1975). Difference in attention to stimulus presentation mode with regard to age.Developmental Psychology, 11, 403–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogosa, D. (1979). Causal models in longitudinal research: Rationale, formulation, and interpretation. In J. R. Nesselroade & P. B. Baltes (Eds.),Longitudinal research in the study of behavior and development (pp. 263–302). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. H., & Allen, T. W. (1978). An examination of attention, arousal, and learning dysfunctions of hyperkinetic children.Psychological Bulletin, 85, 689–715.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, D. M., & Ross, S. A. (1982).Hyperactivity: Current issues, research, and theory (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Routh, D. K., Schroeder, C. S., & O'Tuama, L. A. (1974). Development of activity level in children.Developmental Psychology, 10, 163–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saal, F. E., Downey, R. G., & Lahey, M. A. (1980). Rating the ratings: Assessing the psychometric quality of rating data.Psychological Bulletin, 88, 413–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H. W. (1972).Children's learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trites, R. L., Blouin, A. G. A., & Laprade, K. (1982). Factor analysis of the Conners teacher rating scale based on a large normative sample.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 615–623.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werry, J. S., & Hawthorne, D. (1976). Conners teacher questionnaire—norms and validityAustralian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 10, 257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1973).The study of behavioral development. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was supported by NIH Grant No. 32992.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Milich, R. Cross-sectional and longitudinal observations of activity level and sustained attention in a normative sample. J Abnorm Child Psychol 12, 261–275 (1984). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00910667

Download citation

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00910667

Keywords

Navigation